With linguistics suffering from increasing fractionalization, it is necessary not to lose sight of the overall picture. It seems uncontroversial that the study of language consists of the following five components: processing, use, structure, variation, and change. While some of the relationships between these concepts have been investigated, a systematic integration of these components into a coherent framework is conspicuously missing. A modest attempt is made here to outline such a framework which makes the interrelationships of the components transparent. In all of these components, competition is found to play a key role. At its core, competition is a psycholinguistic effect which arises in the task of selecting an intended unit from among a number of elements concurrently activated in the processing network. The audible and visible outcome of the selection process is language use. Language structure is the prerequisite for competition in that it provides the set of competitors. When competition is low, consistent (i.e., invariant) language use emerges. When competition is high, language use is variable, i.e., synchronic variation occurs. When competition changes over time, language change takes place. Thus, it is language processing in general and competition in particular that constrains and binds together many phenomena of language use, structure, variation, and change.
Aarts, B. (1989). Verb-preposition constructions and small clauses in English. Journal of Linguistics, 251, 277–290.
Ackema, P., & Neeleman, A. (2010). The role of syntax and morphology in compounding. In Sergio Scalise, & Irene Vogel (Eds.), Cross-disciplinary issues in compounding (pp. 21–36). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Altenberg, B. (1982). The genitive vs. the of-construction. A study of syntactic variation in 17th century English. Malmö: CWK Gleerup.
Arndt-Lappe, S. (2014). Synchronic and diachronic analogy in suffix rivalry: The case of -ity and -ness in English. English Language and Linguistics, 181, 497–548.
Arnold, J.E., Wasow, T., Losongco, A., & Ginstrom, R. (2000). Heaviness vs. newness: The effects of structural complexity and discourse status on constituent ordering. Language, 761, 28–55.
Baars, B.J. (1980). The competing plans hypothesis: An heuristic viewpoint on the causes of errors in speech. In H.W. Dechert, & M. Raupach (Eds.), Temporal variables in speech (pp. 39–49). Paris: Mouton.
Baars, B.J. (1992). A dozen competing-plans techniques for inducing predictable slips in speech and action. In B.J. Baars (Ed.), Experimental slips and human error: Exploring the architecture of volition (pp. 129–150). New York: Plenum.
Baars, B.J., & Motley, M.T. (1976). Spoonerisms as sequencer conflicts: Evidence from artificially elicited errors. American Journal of Psychology, 891, 467–484.
Bates, E., et al. (2003). Timed picture naming in seven languages. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 101, 344–380.
Bauer, L. (2006). Competition in English word formation. In A. van Kemenade, & B. Los (Eds.), The handbook of the history of English (pp. 177–198). Oxford: Blackwell.
Berent, I., & Shimron, J. (1997). The representation of Hebrew words: Evidence from the obligatory contour principle. Cognition, 641, 39–72.
Berg, T. (1995). Sound change in child language: A study of inter-word variation. Language and Speech, 381, 331–363.
Berg, T. (1998). The resolution of number conflicts in English and German agreement patterns. Linguistics, 361, 41–70.
Berg, T. (2001). Linguistic structure and change: An explanation from language processing. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Berg, T. (2004). Similarity and contrast in segmental phonology. Linguistics, 421, 1049–1103.
Berg, T., & Neubauer, M. (2014). From unit-and-ten to ten-before-unit order in the history of English numerals. Language Variation and Change, 26, 21–431.
Bock, K., Cutler, A., Eberhard, K.M., Butterfield, S., Cutting, J.C., & Humphreys, K.R. (2006). Number agreement in British and American English: Disagreeing to agree collectively. Language, 821, 64–113.
Bresnan, J., Cueni, A., Nikitina, T., & Baayen, R.H. (2007). Predicting the dative alternation. In G. Bouma, I. Krämer, & J. Zwarts (Eds.), Cognitive foundations of interpretation (pp. 69–94). Amsterdam: Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences.
Bybee, J.L. (2001). Phonology and language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bybee, J., & Scheibman, J. (1999). The effect of usage on degrees of constituency: The reduction of don’t in English. Linguistics, 371, 575–596.
Chen, M.Y. (1972). The time dimension: Contribution toward a theory of sound change. Foundations of Language, 81, 457–498.
Coppock, E. (2010). Parallel grammatical encoding in sentence production: Evidence from syntactic blends. Language and Cognitive Processes, 251, 38–49.
Croft, W. (1990). Typology and universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cutler, A., Hawkins, J.A., & Gilligan, G. (1985). The suffixing preference: a processing explanation. Linguistics, 231, 723–758.
Dell, G.S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 931, 283–321.
Dell, G.S., Burger, L.K., & Svec, W.R. (1997). Language production and serial order: A functional analysis and a model. Psychological Review, 1041, 123–147.
Dell, G.S., & O’Seaghdha, P. (1994). Inhibition in interactive activation models of linguistic selection and sequencing. In D. Dagenbach, & T.H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 409–453). San Diego: Academic Press.
Denison, D. (1998). Syntax. In S. Romaine (Eds.), The Cambridge history of the English language. Vol. 4: 1776–1977 (pp.92–329). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Du Bois, J.W. (1985). Competing motivations. In J. Haiman (Ed.), Iconicity in syntax (pp. 343–365). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Ellegård, A. (1953). The auxiliary do: The establishment and regulation of its use in English. Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell.
Elsness, J. (1984). That or zero? A look at the choice of object clause connective in a corpus of American English. English Studies, 651, 519–533.
Fay, D.A., & Cutler, A. (1977). Malapropisms and the structure of the mental lexicon. Linguistic Inquiry, 81, 505–520.
Ferreira, V.S., & Dell, G.S. (2000). Effect of ambiguity and lexical availability on syntactic and lexical production. Cognitive Psychology, 401, 296–340.
Fromkin, V.A. (1973). Appendix. In V.A. Fromkin (Ed.), Speech errors as linguistic evidence (pp. 243–269). The Hague: Mouton.
Gaskell, M.G., & Dumay, N. (2003). Lexical competition and the acquisition of novel words. Cognition, 891, 105–138.
Goldinger, S.D., Luce, P.A., & Pisoni, D.B. (1989). Priming lexical neighbors of spoken words: Effects of competition and inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language, 281, 501–518.
Goldrick, M., Folk, J.R., & Rapp, B. (2010). Mrs. Malaprop’s neighborhood: Using word errors to reveal neighborhood effects. Journal of Memory and Language, 621, 113–134.
Gordon, J.K. (2002). Phonological neighborhood effects in aphasic speech errors: spontaneous and structured contexts. Brain and Language, 821, 113–145.
Gries, S.T. (1999). Particle movement: A cognitive and functional approach. Cognitive Linguistics, 101, 105–145.
Gries, S.T., & Hilpert, M. (2010). Modeling diachronic change in the third person singular: a multifactorial, verb- and author-specific exploratory approach. English Language and Linguistics, 141, 293–320.
Haiman, J. (1983). Iconic and economic motivation. Language, 591, 781–819.
Haiman, J. (2011). Competing motivations. In Jae Jung Song (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic typology (pp. 148–165). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hawkins, J.A. (1994). A performance theory of order and constituency. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hawkins, J.A. (2004). Efficiency and complexity in grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hay, J., & Plag, I. (2004). What constrains possible suffix combinations? On the interaction of grammatical and processing restrictions in derivational morphology. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 221, 565–596.
Hilpert, M. (2008). The English comparative – language structure and language use. English Language and Linguistics, 121, 395–417.
Hilpert, M. (2013). Constructional change in English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hooper, J.B. (1976). Word frequency in lexical diffusion and the source of morphophonological change. In W.M. Christie Jr. (Ed.), Current progress in historical linguistics (pp. 95–105). Amsterdam: North Holland.
Keenan, E.L., & Comrie, B. (1977). Noun phrase accessibility and universal grammar. Linguistic Inquiry, 81, 63–99.
Kemmer, S., & Barlow, M. (2000). Introduction: A usage-based conception of language. In M. Barlow, & S. Kemmer (Eds.), Usage-based models of language (pp. VII–XXVIII).Stanford: Center for the Study of Language and Information.
Kirby, S. (1997). Competing motivations and emergence: Explaining implicational hierarchies. Linguistic Typology, 11, 5–31.
Kjellmer, G. (2000). Auxiliary marginalities: The case of try. In J.M. Kirk (Ed.), Corpora galore. Analyses and techniques in describing English (pp. 115–124). Amsterdam: Rodopi.
Kroch, A. (1994). Morphosyntactic variation. In K. Bealset al. (Eds.), Papers from the 30th regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society. Vol. 2: The parasession on variation in linguistic theory (pp. 180–201). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Krug, M. (1998). String frequency. Journal of English Linguistics, 261, 286–320.
Kubozono, H. (1990). Phonological constraints on blending in English as a case for phonology-morphology interface. Yearbook of Morphology, 31, 1–20.
Laver, J. (1980). Slips of the tongue as neuromuscular evidence for a model of speech production. In H.D. Dechert, & M. Raupach (Eds.), Temporal variables in speech (pp. 21–26). The Hague: Mouton.
Lieber, R. (1992). Deconstructing morphology. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Lindblom, B., & Maddieson, I. (1988). Phonetic universals in consonant systems. In L.M. Hyman, & C.N. Li (Eds.), Language, speech and mind (pp. 62–78). London: Routledge.
Lindquist, H. (2000). Livelier or more lively? Syntactic and contextual factors influencing the comparison of disyllabic adjectives. In J.M. Kirk (Ed.), Corpora galore. Analyses and techniques in describing English (pp. 125–132). Amsterdam: Rodopi.
Lohmann, A. (2011). Help vs. help to: A multifactorial, mixed-effects account of infinitive marker omission. English Language and Linguistics, 151, 499–521.
Lohse, B., Hawkins, J.A., & Wasow, T. (2004). Domain minimization in English verb-particle constructions. Language, 801, 238–261.
Luick, K. (1964). Historische Grammatik der englischen Sprache. Stuttgart: Bernhard Tauchnitz.
MacKay, D.G. (1969). Forward and backward masking in motor systems. Kybernetik, 61, 57–64.
MacKay, D.G. (1987). The organization of perception and action. New York: Springer.
MacWhinney, B. (1987). The competition model. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 249–308). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
MacWhinney, B. (2005). A unified model of language acquisition. In J.F. Kroll, & A.M.B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism (pp. 49–67). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Malkiel, Y. (1968). The inflectional paradigm as an occasional determinant of sound change. In W.P. Lehmann, & Y. Malkiel (Eds.), Directions for historical linguistics (pp. 21–64). Austin: University of Texas Press.
McCarthy, J.J. (1986). OCP effects: Gemination and antigemination. Linguistic Inquiry, 171, 207–263.
McClelland, J.L., & Elman, J.L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 181, 1–86.
McMillan, C.T., Corley, M., & Lickley, R.J. (2009). Articulatory evidence for feedback and competition in speech production. Language and Cognitive Processes, 241, 44–66.
McQueen, J.M., Norris, D., & Cutler, A. (1994). Competition in spoken word recognition: Spotting words in other words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 201, 621–638.
Mondorf, B. (2010). Variation and change in English resultative constructions. Language Variation and Change, 221, 397–421.
Moscoso del Prado Martín, F., Deutsch, A., Frost, R., Schreuder, R., de Long, N.H., & Baayen, R.H. (2005). Changing places: A cross-linguistic perspective on frequency and family size in Dutch and Hebrew. Journal of Memory and Language, 531, 496–512.
Newmeyer, F.J. (1994). Competing motivations and synchronic analysis. Sprachtypologie und Universalienforschung, 471, 67–77.
Niemi, J., & Laine, M. (1997). Slips of the tongue as linguistic evidence: Finnish word initial segments and vowel harmony. Folia Linguistica, 311, 161–175.
Nooteboom, S.G. (2005). Lexical bias revisited: Detecting, rejecting and repairing speech errors in inner speech. Speech Communication, 471, 43–58.
Norris, D. (1994). Shortlist: A connectionist model of continuous speech recognition. Cognition, 521, 189–234.
Norris, D., McQueen, J.M., & Cutler, A. (1995). Competition and segmentation in spoken-word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 211, 1209–1228.
Panther, K. (1997). Dative alternation from a cognitive perspective. In B. Smieja, & M. Tasch (Eds.), Human contact through language and linguistics (pp. 107–126). Frankfurt a.M.: Peter Lang.
Peterson, R.R., Dell, G.S., & O’Seaghdha, P. (1989). A connectionist model of form-related priming effects. In G.M. Olson, & E.E. Smith (Eds.), Proceedings of the 11th annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 196–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Phillips, B.S. (1984). Word frequency and the actuation of sound change. Language, 601, 320–342.
Pintzuk, S. (1999). Phrase structures in competition. Variation and change in Old English word order. New York: Garland.
Pozdniakov, K., & Segerer, G. (2007). Similar place avoidance: A statistical universal. Linguistic Typology, 111, 307–348.
Riddle, E.M. (1985). A historical perspective on the productivity of the suffixes -ness and -ity. In J. Fisiak (Ed.), Historical semantics. Historical word-formation (pp. 435–461). Berlin: Mouton.
Ronneberger-Sibold, E. (1987). A performance model for a natural theory of linguistic change. In A. G. Ramat, O. Corruba, & G. Bernini (Eds.), Papers from the 7th international conference on historical linguistics (pp. 517–533). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Rosenbach, A. (2002). Genitive variation in English: Conceptual factors in synchronic and diachronic studies. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Schendl, H. (1997). Morphological variation and change in early Modern English: my/mine, thy/thine. In Raymond Hickey, & Stanislaw Puppel (Eds.), Language history and linguistic modelling. Vol. 1: Language history (pp. 179–191). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Schlücker, B., & Matthias, H. (2009). Compounds and phrases. A functional comparison between German A + N compounds and corresponding phrases. Italian Journal of Linguistics, 211, 209–234.
Schlüter, J. (2009a). Consonant or ‘vowel’? A diachronic study of initial <h> from Early Modern English to nineteenth-century English. In D. Minkova (Ed.), Phonological weakness in English (pp. 168–196). Basingstroke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Schlüter, J. (2009b). Weak segments and syllable structure in Middle English. In D. Minkova (Ed.), Phonological weakness in English (pp. 199–236). Basingstroke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Shallice, T., & McGill, J. (1978). The origins of mixed errors. In J. Requin (Ed.), Attention and Performance VII (pp. 193–208). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (1979). Speech error evidence for a serial-ordering mechanism in sentence production. In W.E. Cooper, & Walker E.C.T. (Eds.), Sentence processing: Psycholinguistic studies presented to Merrill Garrett (pp. 295–342). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Slowiaczek, L.M., & Pisoni, D.B. (1986). Effects of phonological similarity on priming in auditory lexical decision. Memory & Cognition, 141, 230–237.
Stemberger, J.P. (1985). An interactive activation model of language production. In A.W. Ellis (Ed.), Progress in the psychology of language (Vol. 11, pp. 143–186). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Stemberger, J.P. (1992). The reliability and replicability of naturalistic speech error data. In B.J. Baars (Ed.), Experimental slips and human error: Exploring the architecture of volition (pp. 195–215). New York: Plenum.
Stemberger, J.P., & MacWhinney, B. (1986). Frequency and the lexical storage of regularly inflected forms. Memory & Cognition, 141, 17–26.
Stemberger, J.P., Pisoni, D.B., & Hathaway, S.N. (1985). Effects of alcohol intoxication on phonological errors in normal speech. Research on Speech Perception, Progress Report, 111, 1–11.
Taylor, J.R. (2003). Linguistic categorization. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Thompson, S.A., & Mulac, A. (1991). The discourse conditions for the use of the complementizer that in conversational English. Journal of Pragmatics, 151, 237–251.
Tottie, G. (1991). Lexical diffusion in syntactic change: Frequency as a determinant of linguistic conservatism in the development of negation in English. In D. Kastovsky (Ed.), Historical English syntax (pp. 439–467). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Trudgill, P. (1972). Sex, covert prestige and linguistic change in the urban British English of Norwich. Language in Society, 11, 179–195.
Ueno, M., & Polinsky, M. (2009). Does headedness affect processing? A new look at the VO-OV contrast. Journal of Linguistics, 451, 675–710.
Ungerer, F. (1998). The choice between adverbs and adverbial phrases. In R. Schulze (Ed.), Making meaningful choices in English (pp. 259–275). Tübingen: Narr.
Vitevitch, M.S. (1997). The neighbourhood characteristics of malapropisms. Language and Speech, 401, 211–228.
Vitevitch, M.S. (2002). Influence of onset density on spoken-word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 281, 270–278.
Vitevitch, M.S., & Stanner, M.K. (2006). The curious case of competition in Spanish speech production. Language and Cognitive Processes, 211, 760–770.
Wang, W.S.-Y. (1969). Competing change as a cause of residue. Language, 451, 9–25.
Wasow, T. (2002). Postverbal behaviour. Stanford: Center for the Study of Language and Information.
Werner, O. (1987). Natürlichkeit und Nutzen morphologischer Irregularität. In N. Boretzky, W. Enninger, & T. Stolz (Eds.), Beiträge zum 3. Essener Kolloquium über Sprachwandel und seine bestimmenden Faktoren (pp. 289–316). Bochum: Brockmeyer.
Wright, R. (2003). Factors of lexical competition in vowel articulation. In J. Local, R. Ogden, & R. Temple (Eds.), Phonetic interpretation. Papers in laboratory phonology VI (pp. 75–87). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Yang, C.D. (2002). Knowledge and learning in natural language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cited by (10)
Cited by ten other publications
Ilioaia, Mihaela
2024. Constructing Meaning: Historical Changes in mihi est and habeo Constructions in Romanian. Languages 9:2 ► pp. 38 ff.
Nagano, Akiko, Alexandra Bagasheva & Vincent Renner
2022. Situation-bound utterances and constructional networks: The evolution of the Hebrew see-farewell family. Lingua 272 ► pp. 103328 ff.
Hartmann, Stefan
2021. Diachronie der Zukunft. Beiträge zur Geschichte der deutschen Sprache und Literatur 143:3 ► pp. 364 ff.
Ungerer, Tobias
2021. Using structural priming to test links between constructions: English caused-motion and resultative sentences inhibit each other. Cognitive Linguistics 32:3 ► pp. 389 ff.
Fonteyn, Lauren
2019. A corpus-based view on the (aspectual-)semantics of Modern English nominalizations. Language Sciences 73 ► pp. 77 ff.
Berg, Thomas
2018. Frequency and serial order. Linguistics 56:6 ► pp. 1303 ff.
Smet, Hendrik De, Frauke D’hoedt, Lauren Fonteyn & Kristel Van Goethem
2018. The changing functions of competing forms: Attraction and differentiation. Cognitive Linguistics 29:2 ► pp. 197 ff.
HOFFMANN, THOMAS
2017. Construction Grammar as Cognitive Structuralism: the interaction of constructional networks and processing in the diachronic evolution of English comparative correlatives. English Language and Linguistics 21:2 ► pp. 349 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 19 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.