Part of
Developing, Modelling and Assessing Second Languages
Edited by Jörg-U. Keßler, Anke Lenzing and Mathias Liebner
[Processability Approaches to Language Acquisition Research & Teaching 5] 2016
► pp. 101120
References (28)
References
Beck, M. (1997). Regular verbs, past tense and frequency: Tracking down a potential source of NS/NNS competence differences. Second Language Research, 13(2), 93-115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., Meisel, J., & Pienemann, M. (1983). Deutsch als Zweitsprache. Der Spracherwerb Ausländischer Arbeiter. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Deng, S. (1992). 《实用粤语教程》。上海:海文音像出版社。[Practical Cantonese Textbook]. Shanghai: Shanghai Audio-Visual Press.Google Scholar
Doughty, C. (2003). Instructed SLA: Constraints, compensation, and enhancement. In C. Doughty & M. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 256-310). Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Doughty, C., & Williams, J. (1998). Issues and terminology . In C. Doughty & J. Williams (Eds.), Focus on Form in Second Language Acquisition (pp. 197-261). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eubank, L., & Grace, S. (1998). V-to-I and inflection and non-native grammars. In M. Beck (Ed.), Morphology and its interface in L2 knowledge (pp. 69-88). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hansen, J. (2001). Linguistic constraints on the acquisition of English syllable codas by native speakers of Mandarin Chinese. Applied Linguistics, 22, 338-365. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, R. (2000). Persistent selective fossilization in second language acquisition and the optimal design of the language faculty. Essex Research Reports in Linguistics, 34, 75-90.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R., & S. Liszka (2003). Locating the source of defective past tense marking in advanced L2 English speakers. In van Hout, R., A. Hulk, F. Kuiken, & R. Towell (Eds.), The interface between syntax and lexicon in second language acquisition (pp. 21-44). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, R. (1995). English syntax: A grammar for English language professionals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. (1998a). Case and tense in the ‘fossilized’ steady state. Second Language Research, 14(3), 1-26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1998b). Dissociating syntax from morphology in a divergent L2 end-state grammar. Second Language Research, 14(3), 359–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2000). Mapping features to forms in second language acquisition. In J. Archibald (Ed.), Second language acquisition and linguistic theory (pp. 102-129). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
. (2003). Second language knowledge of [±past] vs. [±finite]. In Proceedings of the 6th generative approaches to second language acquisition conference (GASLA) (pp. 176-189). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.
. (2008). Feature-assembly in second language acquisition. In Liceras, J., H. Zobl, & H. Goodluck (Eds.), The role of formal features in second language acquisition (pp. 106–140). New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Li, C., & Thompson, S. (1981). Mandarin Chinese: A functional reference grammar. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Li, Y.-H.A. (1990). Order and constituency in Mandarin Chinese. Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Long, M. (1991). Focus on form: A design feature in language teaching. In K. de Bot, R. Ginsbery, & C. Kramsh (Eds.), Foreign language research in cross cultural perspective (pp. 39-52). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Long, M., & Robinson, P. (1998). Focus on form: Theory, research and practice. In C. Doughty & J. Williams (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp. 15-41). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Meisel, J., Clahsen, H., & Pienemann, M. (1981). On determining developmental stages in natural second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 3, 109-135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pallotti, G. (2007). An operational definition of the emergence criterion. Applied Linguistics, 28, 361-382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pienemann, M. (1998). Language processing and second-language development: Processability Theory. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (2007). The stuff of thought. Victoria: The Penguin Group.Google Scholar
Prévost P., & White, L. (2000). Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Second language Research, 16, 103-133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saville-Stroike, M. (2006). Introducing second language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vainikka, A., & Young-Scholten, M. (1994). Direct access to X’-Theory: Evidence from Korean and Turkish adults learning German. In T. Hoekstra & B.D. Schwartz (Eds), Language acquisition studies in generative grammar (pp. 265-316). Amsterdam: Jon Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B. (2007). Input processing in adult second language acquisition. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (pp. 115-136). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
White, L. (2003). Fossilization in steady state L2 grammars: Persistent problems with inflectional morphology. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 129-141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Zhang, Yanyin
2020. The impact of learning context on L2 Chinese word order structure development. Study Abroad Research in Second Language Acquisition and International Education 5:2  pp. 258 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.