Commentary published In:
Pedagogical Linguistics
Vol. 3:2 (2022) ► pp.174180
References (21)
References
DeKeyser, R. M. (1995). Learning second language grammar rules: An experiment with a miniature linguistic system. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17 (3), 379–410. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2005). At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27 (2), 305–352. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (2005). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language. A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27 (2), 141–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gass, S. (1997). Input, interaction, and the second language learner. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gilquin, G. (2021). Using corpora to foster L2 construction learning: A data-driven learning experiment. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 31 (2), 229–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gries, S. T., & Stefanowitsch, A. (2004). Extending collostructional analysis. A corpus-based perspective on ‘alternations’. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 9 (1), 97–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hirata, Y., & Thompson, P. (2021). Communicative data-driven learning: a two-year pilot study. ELT Journal. DOI logo, in press .Google Scholar
Jäschke, K., & Plag, I. (2016). The dative alternation in German-English interlanguage. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38 (3), 485–521. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jenset, G. B., McGillivray, B., & Rundell, M. (2018). The dative alternation revisited. Fresh insights from contemporary British spoken data. In V. Brezina, R. Love, & K. Aijmer (Eds.), Corpus approaches to contemporary British speech. Sociolinguistic studies of the spoken BNC2014 (pp. 185–208). Oxon and New York: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar. Volume 1: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D. (2003). Teaching language: From grammar to grammaring. Boston, MA: Heinle.Google Scholar
Lin, M. H. (2021). Effects of data-driven learning on college students of different grammar proficiencies: A preliminary empirical assessment in EFL classes. SAGE Open Journal, online, 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lo, M. L., & Marton, F. (2012). Towards a science of the art of teaching: Using variation theory as a guiding principle of pedagogical design. International Journal for Lesson and Learning Studies, 1 (1), 7–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Llopis-García, R. (2010). Why cognitive grammar works in the classroom. A case study of mood selection in Spanish. AILA Review, 23 (1), 72–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Madlener, K. (2015). Frequency effects in instructed second language acquisition. Applications of Cognitive Linguistics, Volume 291. Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nassaji, H. (2017). Grammar acquisition. In S. Loewen & M. Sato (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of instructed language acquisition (pp. 205–223). New York: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ott, G. (2017). Exploring Variation Theory in form-focused language teaching. Teaching the present perfect in upper secondary EFL. CELT Matters (Centre for English Language Teaching), 11, 9–29. [URL]
Pinker, S., & Ullman, M. T. (2002). The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6 (11), 456–474. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J. N. (2012). Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33 (4), 829–856. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B. (1993). Grammar teaching for the acquisition-rich classroom. Foreign Language Annals, 26 (4), 435–450. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wu, M. J., & Ionin, T. (2021). Does explicit instruction affect L2 linguistic competence? An examination with L2 acquisition of English inverse scope. Second Language Research, 1 (31), online. DOI logoGoogle Scholar