What makes The Daily Show with Jon Stewart so successful as social and political satire? Rhetorical theorists and critics have identified several mechanisms for satisfying the show’s satiric and parodic aim, which include parodic polyglossia, contextual clash, and satirical specificity (Waisanen, 2009). We present a unified account of meaning construction that encompasses these three mechanisms within the framework of blended fictive interaction (Pascual, 2002, 2008ab). Satire results from emergent effects of different conceptual configurations that have to be in place to integrate a pastiche of speech whose provenance originates in different and diverse contexts and genres. The integration of contradictory, conceptually disjointed pieces of discourse under the governing structure of the conversation frame accounts for the show’s most conspicuous satirical moments. These imagined interactions highlight facets of the real world for critical commentary. The thick description of an influential Daily Show segment deepens our understanding of contemporary political satire.
2.Conceptual Blending Theory and fictive interaction
3.The Daily Show: A satirical news program
4.Data
5.Analysis
5.1‘Addressing’ a real contemporary language user in a present-past reality blend
5.2‘Addressing’ a real contemporary language user in a present reality-fiction blend
5.3‘Speaking’ for a real contemporary language user in a present reality-past counterfactual blend
5.4‘Speaking’ for a real now-deceased language user in a counterfactual past-present reality-fiction blend
5.5Quoting a real contemporary speaker, fictively addressing another real contemporary language user in a past-present reality-nursery rhyme-advertisement blend
Amarasingam, A. (Ed.). (2011). The Stewart/Colbert effect: Essays on the real impacts of fake news. Jefferson, NC: McFarland & Company Inc.
Andersen, P. (1999). Nonverbal communication: Forms and functions. Mountain View, CA: Mayfield Pub Co.
Armstrong, J. K. (2015, August6). How Jon Stewart changed the world. Retrieved May 20, 2017, from [URL]
Baker, P. (2012, October17). For the President, punch, punch, another punch. New York Times. Retrieved from [URL]
Bakhtin, M. (1981). The dialogic imagination: Four essays. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Bakhtin, M. (1986). Speech genres and other late essays. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Baum, M. A. (2002). Sex, lies, and war: How soft news brings foreign policy to the inattentive public. The American Political Science Review, 96(1), 91–109.
Baum, M. A. (2003). Soft news and political knowledge: Evidence of absence or absence of evidence?Political Communication, 201, 173–190.
Baum, M. A. (2005). Talking the vote: Why presidential candidates hit the talk show circuit. American Journal of Political Science, 49(2), 213–234.
Baum, M. A., & Jamison, A. S. (2006). The Oprah effect: How soft news helps inattentive citizens vote consistently. The Journal of Politics, 68(4), 946–959.
Baumgartner, J., & Morris, J. S. (2006). The Daily Show effect candidate evaluations, efficacy, and American youth. American Politics Research, 34(3), 341–367.
Baym, G. (2005). The Daily Show: Discursive integration and the reinvention of political journalism. Political Communication, 22(3), 259–276.
Becker, A. B., Xenos, M. A., & Waisanen, D. J. (2010). Sizing up The Daily Show: Audience perceptions of political comedy programming. Atlantic Journal of Communication, 18(3), 144–157.
Blake, A. (2012, October4). What Google can tell us about the first debate – in 4 charts. The Washington Post. Retrieved from [URL]
Brewer, P. R., & Marquardt, E. (2007). Mock news and democracy: Analyzing The Daily Show. Atlantic Journal of Communication, 15(4), 249–267.
Brône, G., Feyaerts, K., & Veale, T. (2006). Introduction: Cognitive linguistic approaches to humor. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 19(3), 203–228.
Cao, X. (2010). Hearing it from Jon Stewart: The impact of The Daily Show on public attentiveness to politics. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 22(1), 26–46.
CBS Interactive. (2012, October3). CBS news instant poll: Romney wins first presidential debate. CBS News. CBS. Retrieved from [URL]
CNN Politics. (2012, October3). CNN Poll: Most watchers say Romney debate winner. Retrieved from [URL]
Coulson, S. (1996). The Menendez brothers’ virus: Analogical mapping in blended spaces. In A. Goldberg (Ed.), Conceptual structure, discourse and language (pp. 67–81). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Coulson, S. (2001). What’s so funny?: Conceptual integration in humorous examples. Retrieved from [URL]
Coulson, S., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2006). Looking back: Joke comprehension and the space structuring model. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 19(3), 229–250.
CPD. (2012). 2012 Debates. Retrieved May 28, 2018, from [URL]
D’Addario, D. (2015). Jon Stewart helped launch the careers of these Daily Show correspondents. Time Magazine. Retrieved from [URL]
Davis, C. J., Bowers, J. S., & Memon, A. (2011). Social influence in televised election debates: A potential distortion of democracy. PLoS One, 6(3), e18154.
Džanić, N., & Berberoviü, S. (2010). On politicians in big women’s sunglasses driving buses with their feet in mouths: Late-night political humour and conceptual integration theory. Jezikoslovlje, 11(2), 197–214.
Fauconnier, G. ([1985] 1994). Mental Spaces: aspects of meaning construction in natural languages. (2nd edition). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fauconnier, G. (1997). Mappings in thought and language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (1994). Conceptual projection and middle spaces (Technical Report No. 9401). UCSD Cognitive Science.
Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2002). The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York: Basic Books.
Feldman, L. (2013). Learning about politics from The Daily Show: The role of viewer orientation and processing motivations. Mass Communication and Society, 16(4), 586–607.
Feldman, L. (2017). Assumptions about science in satirical news and late-night comedy. In K. H. Jamieson, D. Kahan & D. A. Scheufele (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of the science of science communication (pp. 321–331). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Fillmore, C. J. (1982). Frame Semantics. In The Linguistic Society of Korea (Ed.), Linguistics in the morning calm: International conference on linguistics: Selected papers. Seoul Korea: Hanshin Pub. Co.
Finkelstein, S. (2015, July30). Jon Stewart, superboss. Harvard Business Reveiw. Retrieved from [URL]
Finkelstein, S. (2016). Superbosses: How exceptional leaders master the flow of talent. New York: Portfolio/Penguin.
Fonseca, P. (2016). Fictive interaction blended networks in The Daily Show with Jon Stewart: Conceptualizing political humor discourse not only for entertainment purposes. (Unpublished Doctoral Thesis). University of Salamanca, Spain.
Goffman, E. (1963). Behaviour in public places: Notes on the social organisation of gatherings. New York: Free Press.
Goffman, E. (1981). Footing. In Forms of talk (pp. 124–159). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Goodnow, T. (2011). The Daily Show and rhetoric. Lanham: Lexington Books.
Gray, J., Jones, J. P., & Thompson, E. (2009). The state of satire, the satire of state. In J. Gray, J. P. Jones, & E. Thompson (Eds.), Satire TV politics and comedy in the post-network era. New York & London: New York University.
Jefferson, G. (1984). Transcription notation. In J. Atkinson & J. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of social interaction (pp. ix–xvi). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Jones, J. (2007). ‘Fake’ news versus ‘real’ news as sources of political information: The Daily Show and postmodern political reality. In K. Riegert (Ed.), Politicotainment: Television’s take on the real (pp. 129–149). New York: Peter Lang AG.
Jones, J. (2010). Entertaining politics satiric television and political engagement (2nd ed.). Lanham, Maryland: Rowman & Littlefield.
Knappenberger, B. (2014, June3). Bloomberg news. Jon Stewart’s story: How the fake newsman won over America. Bloomberg. Retrieved from [URL]
Kraus, S. (2000). Televised presidential debates and public policy. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Langacker, R. W. (2001). Discourse in Cognitive Grammar. Cognitive Linguistics, 12(2), 143–188.
Lichter, S. R. (Ed.). (2008). The comedy campaign: The role of late-night TV shows in campaign ’08. Media Monitor, XXII(3), 1–7.
Liddell, S. (1995). Real, surrogate and token space: Grammatical consequences in ASL. In K. Emmorey & J. Reilly (Eds.), Language, gesture and space (pp. 19–41). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Liddell, S. (2003). Grammar, gesture, and meaning in American sign language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Marín-Arrese, J. (2003). Humour as ideological struggle: The view from Cognitive Linguistics. Presented at the Cognitive Linguistics Approaches to Humour, University of La Rioja, Spain. Retrieved from [URL]
Marín-Arrese, J. (2008). Cognition and culture in political cartoons. Intercultural Pragmatics, 5(1), 1–18.
Oakley, T. (2009). From attention to meaning: Explorations in semiotics, lingsuistics, and rhetoric. Bern: Peter Lang Verlag.
Oakley, T., & Pascual, E. (2017). Conceptual blending theory. In B. Dancygier (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 423–448). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Parrill, F. (2012). Interactions between discourse status and viewpoint in co-speech gesture. In B. Dancygier & E. Sweetser (Eds.), Viewpoint in language: A multimodal perspective (pp. 97–112). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Pascual, E. (2002). Imaginary trialogues: Conceptual blending and fictive interaction in criminal courts. Utrecht: LOT.
Pascual, E. (2006). Fictive interaction within the sentence: A communicative type of fictivity in grammar. Cognitive Linguistics, 17(2), 245–267.
Prior, M. (2003). Any good news in soft news? The impact of soft news preference on political knowledge. Political Communication, 201, 149–171.
Sanders, T., Sanders, J., & Sweetser, E. (2009). Causality, cognition and communication: A mental space analysis of subjectivity in causal connectives. In T. Sanders & E. Sweetser (Eds.), Causal categories in discourse and cognition (pp. 19–60). Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Talmy, L. (2000). Fictive motion in language and “ception.” In Toward a cognitive semantics volume I: Concept structuring systems (pp. 99–175). Cambridge, MA.: MIT Press.
Thussu, D. K. (2007). New as entertainment: The rise of global infotainment. London: Sage Publications Ltd.
Turner, M. (2010). Ten lectures on mind and language. Beijing: Foreign Language Teaching and Research Press.
Waisanen, D. J. (2009). A citizen’s guides to democracy inaction: Jon Stewart and Stephen Colbert’s comic rhetorical criticism. Southern Communication Journal, 74(2), 119–140.
Warner, J. (2007). Political culture jamming: The dissident humor of The Daily Show with Jon Stewart. Popular Communications, 5(1), 17–36.
Xenos, M. A., & Becker, A. B. (2009). Moments of zen: Effects of The Daily Show on information seeking and political learning. Political Communication, 26(3), 317–332.
Young, D. G. (2008). The Daily Show as the new journalism. In J. C. Baumgartner & J. S. Morris (Eds.), Laughing matters: Humor and American politics in the media age (pp. 242–259). New York: Routledge.
Young, D. G. (2013). Laughter, learning, or enlightenment? Viewing and avoidance motivations behind The Daily Show and The Colbert Report. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 57(2), 153–169.
Zinoman, J. (2014, April16). True king of late night? He might raise eyebrows image. New York Times. Retrieved from [URL]
Cited by (4)
Cited by four other publications
Burgers, Christian
2023. Irony and Satire. In The Cambridge Handbook of Irony and Thought, ► pp. 325 ff.
Burgers, Christian & Britta C. Brugman
2022. How Satirical News Impacts Affective Responses, Learning, and Persuasion: A Three-Level Random-Effects Meta-Analysis. Communication Research 49:7 ► pp. 966 ff.
[no author supplied]
2023. Irony, Affect, and Related Figures. In The Cambridge Handbook of Irony and Thought, ► pp. 235 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 26 december 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.