Part of
Implicit and Explicit Learning of Languages
Edited by Patrick Rebuschat
[Studies in Bilingualism 48] 2015
► pp. 301324
References
Abdel Rahman, R., van Turennot, M., & Levelt, W.J.M
(2003) Phonologogical encoding is not contigent on semantic feature retrieval: An electrophysiological study on object naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29(5), 850–860Google Scholar
Akamatsu, N
(2009) The effects of training on automatization of word recognition in English as a foreign language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 178–193.Google Scholar
Ammar, A., & Spada, N
(2006) One size fits all?: Recasts, prompts, and L2 learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28(4), 543–574. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J.R
(1983) The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(1987) Skill acquisition: Compilation of weak-method problem situations. Psychological Review, 94(2), 192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1992) Automaticity and the ACT theory. The American Journal of Psychology, 105, 165–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burt, M., Dulay, H., & Hernandez-Chavez, E
(1975) Bilingual syntax measure. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovitch.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R.M
(1996) Exploring automatization processes. Tesol Quarterly, 30(2), 349–357. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997) Beyond explicit rule learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19(2), 195–221. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P.E
(2010) Uses of eye-tracking data in second language sentence processing research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 30(1), 149–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elder, C
(2009) Validating a test of metalinguistic knowledge. In R. Ellis, et al. (Eds.), (pp. 113–138).
Ellis, N.C
(2002) Frequency effects in language processing: A review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 143–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R
(1994) A theory of instructed second language acquisition. In N. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 79–114). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
(2004) The definition and measurement of L2 explicit knowledge. Language Learning, 54(2), 227–275. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2), 141–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) The differential effects of corrective feedback on two grammatical structures. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A series of empirical studies (pp. 339–360). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, R., Loewen, S., Elder, C., Erlam, R., Philp, J., & Reinders, H
(2009) Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language learning, testing, and teaching. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Erlam, R
(2005) Language aptitude and its relationship to instructional effectiveness in second language acquisition. Language Teaching Research, 9(2), 147–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Felix, S.W
(1981) The effect of formal instruction on second language acquisition. Language Learning, 31(1), 87–112. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Felser, C
(2005) Experimental psycholinguistic approaches to second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 21(2), 95–97. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernandez, C
(2008) Reexamining the role of explicit information in processing instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30(3), 277–305. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A.D., Gunter, T., Hahne, A., & Mauth, K
(2004) The relative timing of syntactic and semantic processes in sentence comprehension. NeuroReport, 15(1), 165–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gillon Dowens, M., Guo, T., Guo, J., Barber, H., & Carreiras, M
(2011) Gender and number processing in Chinese learners of Spanish -Evidence from Event Related Potentials. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1651–1659. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gillon Dowens, M., Vergara, M., Barber, H., & Carreiras, M
(2009) Morphosyntactic processing in late second-language learners. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(8), 1870–1887. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hahne, A., & Friederici, A.D
(1999) Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: Early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(2), 194–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hahne, A., Mueller, J.L., & Clahsen, H
(2006) Morphological processing in a second language: Behavioral and event-related brain potential evidence for storage and decomposition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(1), 121–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Han, Y., & Ellis, R
(1998) Implicit knowledge, explicit knowledge and general language proficiency. Language Teaching Research, 2(1), 1–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Handy, T.C
(Ed.) (2005) Event-related potentials: A methods handbook. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hulstijn, J.H., Van Gelderen, A., & Schoonen, R
(2009) Automatization in second language acquisition: What does the coefficient of variation tell us? Applied Psycholinguistics, 30(4), 555–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N
(2011) Conducting reaction time research in second language studies. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kaan, E
(2007) Event-related potentials and language processing: A brief overview. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1(6), 571–591. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaan, E., Harris, A., Gibson, E., & Holcomb, P
(2000) The P600 as an index of syntactic integration. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(2), 159–201. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krashen, S.D
(1981) Second language acquisition and second language learning. New York, NY: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
(1985) The input hypothesis: Issues and implications. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Lachaud, C.M., & Renaud, O
(2011) A tutorial for analyzing human reaction times: How to filter data, manage missing values, and choose a statistical model. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32(2), 389–416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lado, B., Bowden, H., Stafford, C.A., & Sanz, C
(2013) A fine-grained analysis of the effects of negative evidence with and without metalinguistic information in language development. Language Teaching Research, 18(3), 320–344 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Larsen- Freeman, D.E
(1975) The acquisition of grammatical morphemes by adult ESL students. Tesol Quarterly, 9(4), 409–419. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leung, J.H.C., & Williams, J.N
(2011a) The implicit learning of mappings between forms and contextually derived meanings. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 33–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011b) Constraints on implicit learning of grammatical form-meaning connections. Language Learning, 1–29.Google Scholar
Li, S
(2010) The effectiveness of corrective feedback in SLA: A meta-analysis. Language Learning, 60(2), 309–365. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, W. & Moyer, M.G
(2008). Research methods in bilingualism and multilingualism. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Long, M.H
(1983) Does instruction make a difference? Tesol Quarterly, 17, 357–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luck, S.J
(2005) An introduction to the event-related potential technique. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lyster, R
(2004) Differential effects of prompts and recasts in form-focused instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 399–432. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lyster, R., & Izquierdo, J
(2009) Prompts versus recasts in dyadic interaction. Language Learning, 59(2), 453–498. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marinis, T
(2003) Psycholinguistic techniques in second language acquisition research. Second Language Research, 19(2), 144–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McDonough, K., & Trofimovich, P
(2008) Using priming methods in second language research. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, B
(1987) Second-language acquisition in childhood: School-age children (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, J., Osterhout, L., & Kim, A
(2004) Neural correlates of second-language word learning: Minimal instruction produces rapid change. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 703–704. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Finger, I., Grey, S., & Ullman, M.T
(2012) Second language processing shows increased native-like neural responses after months of no exposure. PLoS ONE, 7(3), e32974. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Sanz, C., Steinhauer, K., & Ullman, M.T
(2010) Second language acquisition of gender agreement in explicit and implicit training conditions: An event-related potential study. Language Learning, 60(1), 154–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Steinhauer, K., Sanz, C., & Ullman, M.T
(2012) Explicit and implicit second language training differentially affect the achievement of native-like brain activation patterns. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(4), 933–947. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nagata, N
(1993) Intelligent computer feedback for second language instruction. Modern Language Journal, 77(3), 330–339. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nagata, N., & Swisher, M.V
(1995) A study of consciousness-raising by computer: The effect of metalinguistic feedback on second language learning. Foreign Language Annals, 28(3), 337–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norris, J.M., & Ortega, L
(2000) Effectiveness of L2 instruction: A research synthesis and quantitative meta-analysis. Language Learning, 50(3), 417–528. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Assessing learner knowledge. In S.M. Gass & A. Mackey (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 573–589). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ojima, S., Nakata, H., & Kakigi, R
(2005) An ERP study on second language learning after childhood: Effects of proficiency. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(8), 1212–1228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ortega, L
(2000) Understanding syntactic complexity: The measurement of change in the syntax of instructed L2 Spanish. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Hawai'i at Manoa.
Ortega, L., Iwashita, N., Rabie, S., & Norris, J.M
in preparation). A multilanguage comparison of measures of sytnactic complexity.
Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P.J
(1992) Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785–806. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Osterhout, L., McLaughlin, J., Pitkänen, I., Frenck-Mestre, C., & Molinaro, N
(2006) Novice learners, longitudinal designs, and event-related potentials: A means for exploring the neurocognition of second language processing. Language Learning, 56(Suppl 1), 199–230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pachella, R.G
(1973) The interpretation of reaction time in information processing research. DTIC Document. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Philp, J., & Tognini, R
(2009) Language acquisition in foreign language contexts and the differential benefits of interaction. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL), 47(3-4): 245–266.Google Scholar
Posner, M.I., & Boies, S.J
(1971) Components of attention. Psychological Review, 78(5), 391- 408. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ratcliff, R
(1993) Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological bulletin, 114(3), 510. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reber, A.S
(1967) Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 855–863. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1976) Implicit learning of synthetic languages: The role of instructional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2(1), 88–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberts, L
(2012) Review article: Psycholinguistic techniques and resources in second language acquisition research. Second Language Research, 28(1), 113–127. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P
(1997) Generalizability and automaticity of second language learning under implicit, incidental, enhanced, and instructed conditions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 223–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P.J., & Ha, M.A
(1993) Instance theory and second language rule learning under explicit conditions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 15(04), 413–438. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rossi, S., Gugler, M.F., Friederici, A.D., & Hahne, A
(2006) The impact of proficiency on syntactic second-language processing of German and Italian: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(12), 2030–2048. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roth, D.A.E., Kishon-Rabin, L., Hildesheimer, M., & Karni, A
(2005) A latent consolidation phase in auditory identification learning: Time in the awake state is sufficient. Learning & Memory, 12(2), 159–164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sabourin, L., & Stowe, L.A
(2008) Second language processing: when are first and second languages processed similarly? Second Language Research, 24(3), 397–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanz, C
(1996) The problem of variation in SLA theory and research. In J.E. Alatis, C.A. Straehle, M. Ronkin, & B. Gallenberger (Eds.), Linguistics, Language Acquisition, and Language Variation: Current Trends and Future Prospects (pp. 236–51). Washington DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
(1997) Experimental tasks in SLA research: Amount of production, modality, memory, and production processes. In W.R. Glass & A.T. Pérez-Lerroux (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on the acquisition of Spanish (Vol. 2; pp. 41–56). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
Sanz, C., Lin, H.J., Lado, B., Bowden, H.W., & Stafford, C.A
(2009) Concurrent verbalizations, pedagogical conditions, and reactivity: Two CALL studies. Language Learning, 59(1), 33–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schiffrin, R.M., & Schneider, W
(1977) Controlled and automatic human information processing, II: Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R
(1992) Awareness and second language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 13(1), 206–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R.W
(1990) The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 112, 17–46.Google Scholar
Segalowitz, N
(2003) Automaticity and second languages. In C.J. Doughty & M.H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 382–408). Malden, MA: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, N., & Freed, B.F
(2004) Context, contact, and cognition in oral fluency acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26(2), 173–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, N.S., & Segalowitz, S.J
(1993) Skilled performance, practice, and the differentiation of speed-up from automatization effects: Evidence from second language word recognition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 369–369. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, S.J., Segalowitz, N.S., & Wood, A.G
(1998) Assessing the development of automaticity in second language word recognition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 53–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serafini, E.J
(2013) Exploring relationships between cognitive capacities, second language (L2) motivation, and development of implicit/explicit L2 knowledge over time by proficiency level. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Georgetown University.
Spada, N., & Tomita, Y
(2010) Interactions between type of instruction and type of language feature: A meta-analysis. Language Learning, 60(2), 263–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stafford, C.A., Bowden, H.W., & Sanz, C
(2011) Optimizing explicit instruction: Matters of explicitness, practice, and cue learning. Language Learning (Early View).Google Scholar
Steinhauer, K., & Connolly, J.F
(2008) Event-related potentials in the study of language. In B. Stemmer & H.A. Whitaker (Eds.), Handbook of the neuroscience of language. Oxford: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sunderman, G., & Kroll, J.F
(2009) When study abroad fails to deliver: The internal resources threshold effect. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 79–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tokowicz, N., & MacWhinney, B
(2005) Implicit and explicit measures of sensitivity to violations in second language grammar: An event-related potential investigation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2), 173–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van den Brink, D., & Hagoort, P
(2004) The influence of semantic and syntactic context constraints on lexical selection and integration in spoken-word comprehension as revealed by ERPs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(6), 1068–1084. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B
(2004) Input processing in SLA. In B. VanPatten (Ed.), Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2005) Processing instruction. In C. Sanz (Ed.), Mind and context in adult second language acquisition: Methods, theory, and practice (pp. 267–281). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Weber-Fox, C., & Neville, H
(2001) Sensitive periods differentiate processing of open- and closed-class words: an ERP study of bilinguals. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 44(6), 1338–1353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L
(1991) Adverb placement in second language acquisition: Some effects of positive and negative evidence in the classroom. Second Language Research, 7(2), 133–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, J.N
(2005) Learning without awareness. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 269–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 8 other publications

Faretta-Stutenberg, Mandy, Irene Finestrat & Kara Morgan-Short
2023. Chapter 7. Technology in cognitive research. In Methods in Study Abroad Research [Research Methods in Applied Linguistics, 4],  pp. 157 ff. DOI logo
Grey, Sarah, Jessica G. Cox, Ellen J. Serafini & Cristina Sanz
2015. The Role of Individual Differences in the Study Abroad Context: Cognitive Capacity and Language Development During Short‐Term Intensive Language Exposure. The Modern Language Journal 99:1  pp. 137 ff. DOI logo
Grey, Sarah & Kaitlyn M. Tagarelli
2018. Psycholinguistic Methods. In The Palgrave Handbook of Applied Linguistics Research Methodology,  pp. 287 ff. DOI logo
Grey, Sarah, John N. Williams & Patrick Rebuschat
2014. INCIDENTAL EXPOSURE AND L3 LEARNING OF MORPHOSYNTAX. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 36:4  pp. 611 ff. DOI logo
Leonard, Karen Ruth & Christine E. Shea
2017. L2 Speaking Development During Study Abroad: Fluency, Accuracy, Complexity, and Underlying Cognitive Factors. The Modern Language Journal 101:1  pp. 179 ff. DOI logo
Rose, Maya C., Patricia J. Brooks, Arshia K. Lodhi & Angela Cortez
2023. Benefits of Testing and Production for Learning Turkish As a New Language. Language Learning DOI logo
Sepehrinia, Sajjad, Majid Nemati & Ali Akbar Khomijani Farahani
2019. Studies on oral error correction: do they provide counterevidence against nativist arguments?. The Language Learning Journal 47:3  pp. 354 ff. DOI logo
Zalbidea, Janire & Bernard Issa
2023. Chapter 2. Quantitative approaches in L2 research abroad. In Methods in Study Abroad Research [Research Methods in Applied Linguistics, 4],  pp. 33 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 20 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.