This chapter discusses how event-related potential (ERP) methodology and research may contribute to the issue of explicit and implicit learning of second languages (L2s). To start, we provide a general introduction to ERPs and an overview of common language-related ERP components, including information about their identifying characteristics, the underlying processes that they are understood to reflect, and whether they reflect explicit or implicit processing and knowledge. After providing this overview of language-related ERPs, we briefly describe the general pattern of ERP components found for L2 processing. Next, we consider the potential contributions of ERPs to explicit and implicit issues in L2 acquisition, and review the L2 literature more precisely by considering the patterns of ERP effects elicited by more explicit and implicit learning conditions in both laboratory and natural settings. Finally, we offer conclusions about the contributions of current L2 ERP research to issues of implicit and explicit L2 acquisition and make suggestions as to future directions for research.
Baldwin, K.B., & Kutas, M. (1997). An ERP analysis of implicit structured sequence learning. Psychophysiology, 34(1), 74–86. doi.
Barber, H.A., & Carreiras, M. (2005). Grammatical gender and number agreement in spanish: An ERP comparison. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(1), 137–153.
Bartlett, L. (2013). Adjectives that aren't: An ERP-theoretical analysis of adjectives in spanish. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Illinois at Chicago.
Batterink, L., Karns, C.M., & Neville, H. (2012). Dissociable mechanisms supporting awareness: The P300 and gamma in a linguistic attentional blink task. Cerebral Cortex, 22(12), 2733–2744.
Batterink, L., Karns, C.M., Yamada, Y., & Neville, H. (2010). The role of awareness in semantic and syntactic processing: An ERP attentional blink study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(11), 2514–2529.
Batterink, L., & Neville, H.J. (2013a). The human brain processes syntax in the absence of conscious awareness. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(19), 8528–8533.
Batterink, L., & Neville, H.J. (2013b). Implicit and explicit second language training recruit common neural mechanisms for syntactic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(6), 936–951.
Bentin, S. (1987). Event-related potentials, semantic processes, and expectancy factors in word recognition. Brain and Language, 31(2), 308–327.
Bentin, S., & Peled, B. (1990). The contribution of task-related factors to ERP repetition effects at short and long lags. Memory & Cognition, 18(4), 359–366.
Bowden, H.W., Steinhauer, K., Sanz, C., & Ullman, M.T. (2013). Native-like brain processing of syntax can be attained by university foreign language learners. Neuropsychologia, 51(13), 2492–2511.
Chen, L., Shu, H., Liu, Y., Zhao, J., & Li, P. (2007). ERP signatures of subject-verb agreement in L2 learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10(2), 161–174.
Coulson, S., King, J.W., & Kutas, M. (1998a). ERPs and domain specificity: Beating a straw horse. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13(6), 653–672. Retrieved from [URL]
Coulson, S., King, J.W., & Kutas, M. (1998b). Expect the unexpected: Event-related brain response to morphosyntactic violations. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13(1), 21–58. Retrieved from [URL]
Deacon, D., Dynowska, A., Ritter, W., & Grose-Fifer, J. (2004). Repetition and semantic priming of nonwords: Implications for theories of N400 and word recognition. Psychophysiology, 41(1), 60–74.
DeKeyser, R. (2003). Implicit and explicit learning. In C.J. Doughty, & M.H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 313–348). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
DeKeyser, R. (2007). Skill acquisition theory. In B. VanPatten, & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (pp. 97–114). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Doughty, C.J. (2003). Instructed SLA. In C.J. Doughty, & M.H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 256–310). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Ellis, N.C. (2007). The associate-cognitive CREED. In B. VanPatten, & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (pp. 77–95). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Faretta-Stutenberg, M., & Morgan-Short, K. (2011). Learning without awareness reconsidered: A replication of williams (2005)<br />. In G. Granena, J. Koeth, S. Lee-Ellis, A. Lukyanchenko, G. Prieto Botana & E. Rhoades (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the 2010 second language research forum: Reconsidering SLA research, dimensions, and directions (pp. 18–28). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.
Foucart, A., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2011). Grammatical gender processing in L2: Electrophysiological evidence of the effect of L1-L2 syntactic similarity. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(3), 379–399.
Foucart, A., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2012). Can late L2 learners acquire new grammatical features? evidence from ERPs and eye-tracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(1), 226–248. Retrieved from [URL]
Frenck-Mestre, C., Osterhout, L., McLaughlin, J., & Foucart, A. (2008). The effect of phonological realization of inflectional morphology on verbal agreement in french: Evidence from ERPs. Acta Psychologica, 128(3), 528–536. Retrieved from [URL]
Friederici, A.D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78–84.
Friederici, A.D., Steinhauer, K., & Pfeifer, E. (2002). Brain signatures of artificial language processing: Evidence challenging the critical period hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(1), 529–534.
Friederici, A.D., & Frisch, S. (2000). Verb argument structure processing: The role of verb-specific and argument-specific information. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(3), 476–507.
Friederici, A.D., & Mecklinger, A. (1996). Syntactic parsing as revealed by brain responses: First-pass and second-pass parsing processes. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25(1), 157–176.
Gillon Dowens, M., Guo, T., Guo, J., Barber, H.A., & Carreiras, M. (2011). Gender and number processing in chinese learners of spanish - evidence from event related potentials. Neuropsychologia, 49(7), 1651–1659.
Gillon Dowens, M., Vergara, M., Barber, H.A., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Morphosyntactic processing in late second-language learners. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(8), 1870–1887.
Gouvea, A.C., Phillips, C., Kazanina, N., & Poeppel, D. (2010). The linguistic processes underlying the P600. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(2), 149–188.
Gunter, T.C., Stowe, L.A., & Mulder, G. (1997). When syntax meets semantics. Psychophysiology, 34(6), 660–676.
Hagoort, P., Baggio, G., & Willems, R.M. (2009). Semantic unification. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (IV ed., pp. 819–836). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Retrieved from [URL]
Hagoort, P., & Brown, C.M. (1999). Gender electrified: ERP evidence on the syntactic nature of gender processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28(6), 715–728.
Hagoort, P., & Brown, C.M. (2000). ERP effects of listening to speech compared to reading: The P600/SPS to syntactic violations in spoken sentences and rapid serial visual presentation. Neuropsychologia, 38(11), 1531–1549.
Hagoort, P., Brown, C., & Groothusen, J. (1993). The syntactic positive shift (SPS) as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(4), 439–483.
Hahne, A., & Friederici, A.D. (2002). Differential task effects on semantic and syntactic processes as revealed by ERPs. Cognitive Brain Research, 13, 339–356.
Hahne, A., Mueller, J., & Clahsen, H. (2006). Morphological processing in a second language: Behavioral and event-related brain potential evidence for storage and decomposition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(1), 121–134.
Hahne, A. (2001). What's different in second-language processing? evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(3), 251–266.
Hahne, A., Eckstein, K., & Friederici, A.D. (2004). Brain signatures of syntactic and semantic processes during children's language development. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(7), 1302–1318.
Hahne, A., & Friederici, A.D. (1999). Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: Early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(2), 194–205.
Hahne, A., & Friederici, A.D. (2001). Processing a second language: Late learners' comprehension mechanisms as revealed by event-related brain potentials. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4(2), 123–141. Retrieved from [URL]
Hama, M., & Leow, R.P. (2010). Learning without awareness revisited: Extending williams (2005). Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32(3), 465–491.
Holcomb, P.J., Grainger, J., & O'Rourke, T. (2002). An electrophysiological study of the effects of orthographic neighborhood size on printed word perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(6), 938–950.
Holcomb, P.J., & Neville, H.J. (1990). Auditory and visual semantic priming in lexical decision: A comparison using event-related brain potentials. Language and Cognitive Processes, 5(4), 281–312.
Holcomb, P.J., Reder, L., Misra, M., & Grainger, J. (2005). The effects of prime visibility on ERP measures of masked priming. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(1), 155–172.
Isel, F. (2007). Syntactic and referential processes in second-language learners: Event-related brain potential evidence. Neuroreport, 18(18), 1885–1889.
Kaan, E., Harris, A., Gibson, E., & Holcomb, P. (2000). The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(2), 159–201.
Kaan, E. (2007). Event-related potentials and language processing: A brief overview. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1(6), 571–591.
Kappenman, E.S., & Luck, S.J. (2012). ERP components: The ups and downs of brainwave recordings. In S. Luck J., & E.S. Kappenman (Eds.), The oxford handbook of ERP components (pp. 3–30). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kim, A., & Osterhout, L. (2005). The independence of combinatory semantic processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(2), 205–225. Retrieved from [URL]
King, J.W., & Kutas, M. (1995). Who did what and when? using word- and clause-level ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(3), 376–395. Retrieved from [URL]
Kluender, R., & Kutas, M. (1993). Subjacency as a processing phenomenon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(4), 573–633. Retrieved from [URL]
Kluender, R., & Münte, T.F. (1998). ERPS to grammatical and ungrammatical subject/object asymmetries in german wh-questions. [Abstract]. Poster Presented at the CUNY Conference on Sentence Processing, Newark, NJ.
Knowlton, B.J., & Moody, T.D. (2008). Procedural learning in humans. In J. Byrnes (Ed.), Learning & memory: A comprehensive reference, (Vol. 3): Memory systems (pp. 321–340). Oxford: Elsevier.
Kuperberg, G.R. (2007). Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: Challenges to syntax. Brain Research, 1146, 23–49.
Kuperberg, G.R., Sitnikova, T., Caplan, D., & Holcomb, P.J. (2003). Electrophysiological distinctions in processing conceptual relationships within simple sentences. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(1), 117–129.
Kutas, M., Neville, H.J., & Holcomb, P.J. (1987). A preliminary comparison of the N400 response to semabtic anomalies during reading, listening and singing. In R.J. Ellingson, N.M.F. Murray & A.M. Halliday (Eds.), Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology (pp. 325–330). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K.D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463–470.
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K.D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621–647.
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S.A. (1983). Event-related brain potentials to grammatical errors and semantic anomalies. Memory & Cognition, 11(5), 539–550.
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S.A. (1984). Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature, 307(5947), 161–163.
Lau, E.F., Phillips, C., & Poeppel, D. (2008). A cortical network for semantics: (De)constructing the N400. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(12), 920–933.
Leow, R.P. (2000). A study of the role of awareness in foreign language behavior: Aware versus unaware learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22(4), 557–584. Retrieved from [URL]
Leung, J., & Williams, J.N. (2011). The implicit learning of mappings between forms and contextually-derived meanings<br />. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33(1), 33–55.
Leung, J., & Williams, J.N. (2012). Constraints on implicit learning of grammatical form-meaning connections. Language Learning, 62(2), 634–662. Retrieved from [URL]
Luck, S., J. (2005). An introduction to the event-related potential technique. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Martín-Loeches, M., Nigbur, R., Casado, P., Hohlfeld, A., & Sommer, W. (2006). Semantics prevalence over syntax during sentence processing: A brain potential study of noun-adjective agreement in spanish. Brain Research, 1093(1), 178–189.
McCarthy, G., & Nobre, A.C. (1993). Modulation of semantic processing by spatial selective attention. Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology: Evoked Potentials, 88(3), 210–219.
McKinnon, R., & Osterhout, L. (1996). Constraints on movement phenomena in sentence processing: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Language and Cognitive Processes, 11(5), 495–523. Retrieved from [URL]
McLaughlin, J., Osterhout, L., & Kim, A. (2004). Neural correlates of second-language word learning: Minimal instruction produces rapid change. Nature Neuroscience, 7(7), 703–704.
McLaughlin, J., Tanner, D., Pitkänen, I., Frenck-Mestre, C., Inoue, K., Valentine, G., & Osterhout, L. (2010). Brain potentials reveal discrete stages of L2 grammatical learning. Language Learning, 60, 123–150.
Morgan-Short, K., Finger, I., Grey, S., & Ullman, M.T. (2012). Second language processing shows increased native-like neural responses after months of no exposure. Plos One, 7(3), e32974.
Morgan-Short, K., Sanz, C., Steinhauer, K., & Ullman, M.T. (2010). Second language acquisition of gender agreement in explicit and implicit training conditions: An event-related potential study. Language Learning, 60(1), 154–193. Retrieved from [URL]
Morgan-Short, K., Steinhauer, K., Sanz, C., & Ullman, M.T. (2012). Explicit and implicit second language training differentially affect the achievement of native-like brain activation patterns. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(4), 933–947.
Morris, J., & Holcomb, P.J. (2005). Event-related potentials to violations of inflectional verb morphology in english. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(3), 963–981.
Mueller, J.L. (2005). Electrophysiological correlates of second language processing. Second Language Research, 21(2), 152–174.
Mueller, J.L., Hahne, A., Fujii, Y., & Friederici, A.D. (2005). Native and nonnative speakers' processing of a miniature version of japanese as revealed by ERPs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(8), 1229–1244.
Mueller, J.L. (2009). The influence of lexical familiarity on ERP responses during sentence comprehension in language learners. Second Language Research, 25(1), 43–76. Retrieved from [URL]
Mueller, J.L., Hirotani, M., & Friederici, A.D. (2007). ERP evidence for different strategies in the processing of case markers in native speakers and non-native learners. BMC Neuroscience, 8(18)
Mueller, J.L., Oberecker, R., & Friederici, A.D. (2009). Syntactic learning by mere exposure an ERP study in adult learners. BMC Neuroscience, 10(89)
Neville, H., Nicol, J.L., Barss, A., Forster, K.I., & Garrett, M.F. (1991). Syntactically based sentence processing classes - evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(2), 151–165.
Norris, J.M., & Ortega, L. (2000). Effectiveness of L2 instruction: A research synthesis and quantitative meta-analysis. Language Learning, 50(3), 417–528.
Oberecker, R., & Friederici, A.D. (2006). Syntactic event-related potential components in 24-month-olds' sentence comprehension. NeuroReport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 17(10), 1017–1021.
Oberecker, R., Friedrich, M., & Friederici, A.D. (2005). Neural correlates of syntactic processing in two-year-olds. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(10), 1667–1678. Retrieved from [URL]
Ojima, S., Nakata, H., & Kakigi, R. (2005). An ERP study of second language learning after childhood: Effects of proficiency. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(8)
Ortega, L. (2009). Cognition. Understanding second language acquisition (pp. 82–109). London: Hodder Education.
Osterhout, L., Allen, M.D., McLaughlin, J., & Inoue, K. (2002). Brain potentials elicited by prose-embedded linguistic anomalies. Memory & Cognition, 30(8), 1304–1312. Retrieved from [URL]
Osterhout, L., & Hagoort, P. (1999). A superficial resemblance does not necessarily mean you are part of the family: Counterarguments to coulson, king and kutas (1998) in the P600/SPS-P300 debate. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(1), 1–14. Retrieved from [URL]
Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P.J. (1992). Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(6), 785–806. Retrieved from [URL]
Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P.J., & Swinney, D.A. (1994). Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: Evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 786–803. Retrieved from [URL]
Osterhout, L., McKinnon, R., Bersick, M., & Corey, V. (1996). On the language specificity of the brain response to syntactic anomalies: Is the syntactic positive shift a member of the P300 family?Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(6), 507–526. Retrieved from [URL]
Osterhout, L., & Mobley, L.A. (1995). Event-related brain potentials elicited by failure to agree. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(6), 739–773. Retrieved from [URL]
Osterhout, L., Poliakov, A., Inoue, K., McLaughlin, J., Valentine, G., Pitkanen, I., Hirschensohn, J. (2008). Second-language learning and changes in the brain. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(6), 509–521. Retrieved from [URL]
Pakulak, E., & Neville, H.J. (2010). Proficiency differences in syntactic processing of monolingual native speakers indexed by event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(12), 2728–2744.
Pakulak, E., & Neville, H.J. (2011). Maturational constraints on the recruitment of early processes for syntactic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 2752–2765.
Polich, J. (2012). Neuropsychology of P300. In S.J. Luck, & E.S. Kappenman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of event-related potential components (pp. 159–188). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rebuschat, P., Hamrick, P., Sachs, R., Riestenberg, K., & Ziegler, N. (2013). Implicit and explicit knowledge of form-meaning connections: Evidence from subjective measures of awareness. In J. Bergsleithner, S. Frota & J.K. Yoshioka (Eds.), Noticing: L2 studies and essays in honor of Dick Schmidt (pp. 255–275). Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.
Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J.N. (2012). Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33(4), 829–856.
Rolke, B., Heil, M., Streb, J., & Hennighausen, E. (2001). Missed prime words within the attentional blink evoke an N400 semantic priming effect. Psychophysiology, 38(2), 165–174.
Rossi, S., Gugler, M.F., Friederici, A.D., & Hahne, A. (2006). The impact of proficiency on syntactic second-language processing of german and italian: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(12), 2030–2048.
Rugg, M.D., & Coles, M.G.H. (1995). The ERP and cognitive psychology: Conceptual issues. In M.D. Rugg, & M.G.H. Coles (Eds.), Electrophysiology of mind: Event-related brain potentials and cognition (pp. 27–39). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sabourin, L., & Stowe, L.A. (2008). Second language processing: When are first and second languages processed similarly?Second Language Research, 24(3), 397–430.
Sanz, C., & Morgan-Short, K. (2004). Positive evidence versus explicit rule presentation and explicit negative feedback: A computer-assisted study. Language Learning, 54(1), 35–78.
Schmidt, R. (1990). The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 11(2), 129–158. Retrieved from [URL]
Schmidt, R. (1995). Consciousness and foreign language learning: A tutorial on the role of attention and awareness in learning. In R. Schmidt (Ed.), Attenion and awareness in foreign language learning (technical report #9) (pp. 1–63). Honolulu, HI: University of Hawai'i, Second Language Teaching & Curriculum Center.
Spada, N., & Tomita, Y. (2010). Interactions between type of instruction and type of language feature: A meta-analysis. Language Learning, 60(2), 263–308.
Stafford, C.A., Bowden, H.W., & Sanz, C. (2012). Optimizing language instruction: Matters of explicitness, practice, and cue learning. Language Learning, 62(3), 741–768.
Steinhauer, K., & Connolly, J.F. (2008). Event-related potentials in the study of language. In Stemmer, B. & Whitaker, H. (Ed.), Handbook of the cognitive neuroscience of language (pp. 91–104). New York, NY: Elsvevier.
Steinhauer, K., & Drury, J.E. (2012). On the early left-anterior negativity (ELAN) in syntax studies. Brain and Language, 120(2), 135–162.
Steinhauer, K., White, E.J., & Drury, J.E. (2009). Temporal dynamics of late second language acquisition: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Second Language Research, 25(1), 13–41.
Stenberg, G., Lindgren, M., Johansson, M., Olsson, A., & Rosén, I. (2000). Semantic processing without conscious identification: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(4), 973–1004.
Swaab, T.Y., Ledoux, K., Camblin, C.C., & Boudewyn, M.A. (2012). Language related ERP components. In S.J. Luck, & E.S. Kappenman (Eds.), Oxford handbook of event-related potential components. (pp. 397–440). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Tanner, D., & van Hell, J.G. (2014). ERPs reveal individual differences in morphosyntactic processing. Neuropsychologia, 56, 289–301.
Tanner, D., Inoue, K., & Osterhout, L. (2013). Brain-based individual differences in online L2 grammatical comprehension. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, FirstView, 1–17.
Tanner, D., McLaughlin, J., Herschensohn, J., & Osterhout, L. (2013). Individual differences reveal stages of L2 grammatical acquisition: ERP evidence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(2), 367–382.
Tokowicz, N., & MacWhinney, B. (2005). Implicit and explicit measures of sensitivity to violations in second language grammar: An event-related potential investigation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2), 173–204.
Ullman, M.T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92(1-2), 231–270.
van Hell, J.G., & Tokowicz, N. (2010). Event-related brain potentials and second language learning: Syntactic processing in late L2 learners at different L2 proficiency levels. Second Language Research, 26(1), 43–74.
Van Petten, C. (1995). Words and sentences: Event-related brain potential measures. Psychophysiology, 32(6), 511–525.
Vogel, E.K., Luck, S.J., & Shapiro, K.L. (1998). Electrophysiological evidence for a postperceptual locus of suppression during the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(6), 1656–1674.
Voss, J.L., & Paller, K.A. (2009). An electrophysiological signature of unconscious recognition memory. Nature Neuroscience, 12(3), 349–355.
Weber, K., & Lavric, A. (2008). Syntactic anomaly elicits a lexico-semantic (N400) ERP effect in the second language but not the first. Psychophysiology, 45(6), 920–925.
Weber-Fox, C.M., & Neville, H.J. (1996). Maturational constraints on functional specializations for language processing: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilingual speakers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(3), 231–256.
White, E.J., Genesee, F., & Steinhauer, K. (2012). Brain responses before and after intensive second language learning: Proficiency based changes and first language background effects in adult learners. PLoS ONE, 7(12), e52318.
Williams, J.N. (2005). Learning without awareness. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2), 269–304. Retrieved from [URL]
Cited by (17)
Cited by 17 other publications
Xu, Emily Shimeng, Stephen Matthews, Virginia Yip, Patrick C. M. Wong & Nicola Molinaro
2024. Memory systems modulate crosslinguistic influence on third language morphosyntactic acquisition. PLOS ONE 19:7 ► pp. e0304572 ff.
Faretta-Stutenberg, Mandy, Irene Finestrat & Kara Morgan-Short
2023. An fMRI validation study of the word-monitoring task as a measure of implicit knowledge: Exploring the role of explicit and implicit aptitudes in behavioral and neural processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 45:1 ► pp. 109 ff.
Morgan‐Short, Kara, Irene Finestrat, Alicia Luque & David Abugaber
2022. Exploring New Insights Into Explicit and Implicit Second Language Processing: Event‐Related Potentials Analyzed by Source Attribution. Language Learning 72:2 ► pp. 365 ff.
2022. Second Language Vocabulary Learning through Ecologically Valid Classroom Practice Can Be Indexed by Event‐Related Potentials (ERPs). A Conceptual Replication Study. Mind, Brain, and Education 16:2 ► pp. 122 ff.
Morgan‐Short, Kara
2021. Considering the updated Input Hypothesis from a neurolinguistic perspective: A response to Lichtman and VanPatten. Foreign Language Annals 54:2 ► pp. 324 ff.
Yilmaz, Yucel & Gisela Granena
2021. IMPLICITNESS AND EXPLICITNESS IN COGNITIVE ABILITIES AND CORRECTIVE FEEDBACK. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 43:3 ► pp. 523 ff.
Angelovska, Tanja & Dietmar Roehm
2020. Neurolinguistic Implications for L2 Learning and Teaching. In The TESOL Encyclopedia of English Language Teaching, ► pp. 1 ff.
Granena, Gisela
2020. Implicit Language Aptitude,
Mackey, Alison
2020. Interaction, Feedback and Task Research in Second Language Learning,
Curcic, Maja, Sible Andringa & Folkert Kuiken
2019. The Role of Awareness and Cognitive Aptitudes in L2 Predictive Language Processing. Language Learning 69:S1 ► pp. 42 ff.
Roehm, Dietmar & Tanja Angelovska
2019. Neurolinguistic Accounts ofL2Learning and Teaching. In The TESOL Encyclopedia of English Language Teaching, ► pp. 1 ff.
Faretta-Stutenberg, Mandy & Kara Morgan-Short
2018. The interplay of individual differences and context of learning in behavioral and neurocognitive second language development. Second Language Research 34:1 ► pp. 67 ff.
Loewen, Shawn & Masatoshi Sato
2018. Interaction and instructed second language acquisition. Language Teaching 51:3 ► pp. 285 ff.
Suzuki, Yuichi & Robert DeKeyser
2017. The Interface of Explicit and Implicit Knowledge in a Second Language: Insights From Individual Differences in Cognitive Aptitudes. Language Learning 67:4 ► pp. 747 ff.
Andringa, Sible & Patrick Rebuschat
2015. NEW DIRECTIONS IN THE STUDY OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT LEARNING. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 37:2 ► pp. 185 ff.
Morgan-Short, Kara, ZhiZhou Deng, Katherine A. Brill-Schuetz, Mandy Faretta-Stutenberg, Patrick C. M. Wong & Francis C. K. Wong
2015. A VIEW OF THE NEURAL REPRESENTATION OF SECOND LANGUAGE SYNTAX THROUGH ARTIFICIAL LANGUAGE LEARNING UNDER IMPLICIT CONTEXTS OF EXPOSURE. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 37:2 ► pp. 383 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 30 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.