Part of
Growing Old with Two Languages: Effects of Bilingualism on Cognitive Aging
Edited by Ellen Bialystok and Margot D. Sullivan
[Studies in Bilingualism 53] 2017
► pp. 920
References
Anguera, J. A., Boccafuso, J., Rintoul, J. L., Al-Hashimi, O., Faraji, F., Janowich, J., Kong, E., Larraburo, Y., Rolle, C., Johnston, E. & Gazzaley, A.
(2013) Video-game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature, 501, 97–101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2001) Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge, UK: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Coordination of executive functions in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(3), 461–468. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Craik, F. I. M.
(2007) Bilingualism and naming: Implications for cognitive assessment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 209–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Binns, M. A., Ossher, L., & Freedman, M.
(2014) Effects of bilingualism on the age of onset and progression of MCI and AD: evidence from executive function tests. Neuropsychology, 28, 290–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G.
(2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 34, 828–828. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Luk, G.
(2011) Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 397–401. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., & Viswanathan, M.
(2005) Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9, 103–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Poarch, G., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. M.
(2014) Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging, 29(3), 696–705. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V.
(2011) Bilingualism influences inhibitory control in auditory comprehension. Cognition, 118, 245–257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Braver, T. S., & West, R.
(2008) Working memory, executive control, and aging. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (3rd ed., pp. 311–372). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Burke, S. N. & Barnes, C. A.
(2010) Senescent synapses and hippocampal circuit dynamics. Trends in Neurosciences, 33(3), 153–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R. B.
(1971) Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Colzato, L. S., Bajo, M. T., van den Wildenberg, W., Paolieri, D., Nieuwenhuis, S., La Heij, W., & Hommel, B.
(2008) How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 34, 302–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Conway, A. R., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W.
(2005) Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 769–786. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, A., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 59–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I. M.
(1971) Primary memory. British Medical Bulletin, 27, 232–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1986) A functional account of age differences in memory. In F. Klix, & H. Hagendorf (Eds.), Human memory and cognitive capabilities. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., & Bialystok, E.
(2006) Cognition through the lifespan: Mechanisms of change. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 131–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I. M.
(1983) On the transfer of information from temporary to permanent memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 302, 341–359. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Fennema-Notestine, C., & Morris, S. K.
(2005) Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory & Cognition, 33, 1220–1234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., & Silverberg, N. B.
(2001) Tip-of-the-tongue states in Hebrew – English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 63–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goral, M., Campanelli, L., & Spiro, A.
(2015) Language dominance and inhibition abilities in bilingual older adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 79–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W.
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J.
(2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 515–530. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T.
(1979) Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 356–388. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L., Zacks, R. T., & May, C. P.
(1999) Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. In D. Gopher & A. Koriat (Eds.), Attention and Performance XVII (pp. 653–675). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Horn, J. L.
(1982) The theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence in relation to concepts of cognitive psychology and aging in adulthood. In F. I. M. Craik & S. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes (pp. 237–278). New York, NY: Plenum Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacoby, L. L.
(1991) A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–541. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
James, W.
(1890) The principles of psychology. New York, NY: H. Holt and Company. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jennings, J. M., & Jacoby, L. L.
(1993) Automatic versus intentional uses of memory: Aging, attention, and control. Psychology and Aging, 8, 283–293. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kousaie, S., & Phillips, N. A.
(2012) Ageing and bilingualism: Absence of a “bilingual advantage” in stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 356–369. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Dijkstra, T.
(2002) The bilingual lexicon. In R. B. Kaplan (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of applied linguistics (pp. 301–321). Oxford, UK: OUP.Google Scholar
Luk, G., Anderson, J. A., Craik, F. I. M., Grady, C., & Bialystok, E.
(2010) Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition versus interference suppression. Brain and Cognition, 74, 347–357. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luo, L., Craik, F. I. M., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging, 28, 28–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E.
(2010) Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 114, 29–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E.
(2008) The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 81–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Naveh-Benjamin, M.
(2000) Adult age differences in memory performance: Tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 1170–1187.Google Scholar
Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J. A., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A. S., Howard, R. J. & Ballard, C. G.
(2010) Putting brain training to the test. Nature, 465, 775–778. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pelham, S. D., & Abrams, L.
(2014) Cognitive advantages and disadvantages in early and late bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 313–325. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poarch, G. J., & Van Hell, J. G.
(2012) Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 535–551. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Portocarrero, J., Burright, R., & Donovick, P.
(2007) Vocabulary and verbal fluency of bilingual and monolingual college students. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 415–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rogers, C. L., Lister, J. J., Febo, D. M., Besing, J. M., & Abrams, H. B.
(2006) Effects of bilingualism, noise, and reverberation on speech perception by listeners with normal hearing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 465–485. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A.
(1982) Adult cognition: An experimental psychology of human aging. New York, NY: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996) The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schroeder, S. R., & Marian, V.
(2012) A bilingual advantage for episodic memory in older adults. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 591–601. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, M. C.
(1997) How do bilinguals access lexical information? In A. M. Groot & J. F. Kroll (Eds.), Tutorials in bilingualism: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp. 145–168). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Soveri, A., Laine, M., Hämäläinen, H., & Hugdahl, K.
(2010) Bilingual advantage in attentional control: Evidence from the forced-attention dichotic listening paradigm. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 371–378. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M.
(2011) Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 183. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tong, H. C., Werner, R. A. & Franzblau, A.
(2004) Effect of aging on sensory nerve conduction study parameters. Muscle & Nerve, 29, 716–720. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Waugh, N. C., & Norman, D. A.
(1965) Primary memory. Psychological Review, 72, 89–104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wodniecka, Z., Craik, F. I. M., Luo, L., & Bialystok, E.
(2010) Does bilingualism help memory? Competing effects of verbal ability and executive control. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13, 575–595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Armstrong, Bonnie A, Natalie Ein, Brenda I Wong, Sara N Gallant & Lingqian Li
2021. Retracted: The Effect of Bilingualism on Older Adults’ Inhibitory Control: A Meta-Analysis. The Gerontologist 61:3  pp. e102 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.