Part of
Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond: Questions and insights
Edited by Irina A. Sekerina, Lauren Spradlin and Virginia Valian
[Studies in Bilingualism 57] 2019
► pp. 147160
References
Barac, R. & Bialystok, E.
(2012) Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: Role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Development, 83(2), 413–422.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S.
(2013) Package ‘lme4’.Google Scholar
Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J.
(2007) Shared syntactic representations in bilinguals: Evidence for the role of word-order repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 931–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(1988) Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. Developmental Psychology, 24, 560–567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. & Martin, M. M.
(2004) Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7(3), 325–339. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2009) Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(1), 3–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, K.
(1986) Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P.
(2007) Syntactic priming. Language and Linguistic Compass, 1(1–2), 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., McLean, J. F., & Jones, M. G.
(2005) A blue cat or a cat that is blue? Evidence for abstract syntax in young children’s noun phrases. In A. Brugos, M. R. Clark-Cotton, & S. Ha, The proceedings of the 29th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 109–121). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Cai, Z., Pickering, M., & Branigan, H. P.
(2011) Lexical and syntactic representations in closely related languages: Evidence from Cantonese-Mandarin bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 65(4), 421–445. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N.
(2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science 11(2), 282–298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cattaneo, G., Calabria, M., Marne, P., Gironell, A., Abutalebi, J., & Costa, A.
(2015) The role of executive control in bilingual language production: A study with Parkinson’s Disease patients. Neuropsychologia, 66, 99–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunn, Lloyd M., Dunn, Leota M., Wheton, C., & Burtlet, J.
(2007) The British picture vocabulary scale (2nd ed.). London: GL Assessment Limited.Google Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E.
(2004) Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish-English bilinguals. Psychological Science, 6(15), 409–414. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hervé, C., Serratrice, L., & Corley, M.
(2015) Dislocations in French-English bilingual children: An elicitation study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(5), 987–1000. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok
(2013) Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 497–514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lyster Haalas, S. A., Horn, E., & Rygvold, A. E.
(2010) Ordforråd og ordforråd utvikling hos Norske barn og unge. Resultater fra en utprøving av British Picture Vocabulary Scale II (2nd ed.) (BPVS II). Spesialpedagogikk, 74(9), 35–43.Google Scholar
Miyake, A. & Friedman, N. P.
(2012) The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morales, J., Gómez-Ariza, C., & Bajo, M. T.
(2013) Dual mechanisms of cognitive control in bilinguals and monolinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 531–546. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mukadam, N., Sommerland, A., & Livingston, G.
(2017) The relationship of bilingualism compared to monolingualism to the risk of cognitive decline or dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Alzheimers Disease, 58(1), 45–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicolay, A. C. & Poncelet, M.
(2013) Cognitive abilities underlying second-language vocabulary acquisition in an early second-language immersion education context: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(4), 655–671. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peristeri, E., Tsimpli, I. M., Sorace, A., & Tsapkini, K.
(2017) Language interference and inhibition in early and late successive bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schoonbaert, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J.
(2007) The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(2), 153–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shallice, T.
(1988) From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A.
(2016) Language and cognition in bilingual production: The real work still lies ahead. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tungseth, M. E.
(2006) Verb preposition in Norwegian: Paths, places and possession (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). UiT, the Arctic University of Norway.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S.
(2017) Bilingual language experience calculator. Nijmegen: Radboud University Nijmegen.Google Scholar
Valian, V.
(2015) Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 3–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H., Gámez, P. B., Gómez, L. E., Bowers, E., & Shimpi, P.
(2010) Cross-linguistic structural priming in bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 37, 1047–1064. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wolleb, A.
(2015) Syntactic representations in the bilingual mind: The role of executive function and pragmatics in cross-language priming (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). UiT, the Artctic University of Norway.Google Scholar
Wolleb, A., Sorace, A., & Westergaard, M.
Zelazo, P. D., & Frye, D.
(1998) Cognitive complexity and control: The development of executive function in childhood. Current Direction in Psychological Science, 4(7), 121–126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Rosso, Aline Casagrande, Ana Karina Paz Pereira & Ana Beatriz Arêas da Luz Fontes
2021. A relação entre leitura em L2 e funções executivas. Cadernos de Linguística 2:4  pp. e442 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 23 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.