Part of
Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond: Questions and insights
Edited by Irina A. Sekerina, Lauren Spradlin and Virginia Valian
[Studies in Bilingualism 57] 2019
► pp. 237246
References
Babcock, L., & Vallesi, A.
(2017) Are simultaneous interpreters expert bilinguals, unique bilinguals, or both? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(2), 403–417. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chertkow, H., Whitehead, V., Phillips, N., Wolfson, C., Atherton, J., & Bergman, H.
(2010) Bilingualism (but not always bilingualism) delays the onset of Alzheimer disease: evidence from a bilingual community. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 24 (2), 118–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I., Bialystok, E. & Freedman, M.
(2010) Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology, 75 (19), 1726–1729. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Bruin, A., Treccani, B., & Della Sala, S.
(2015) Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychological Science, 26, 99–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Vries, G. J.
(2004) Minireview: sex differences in adult and developing brains: compensation, compensation, compensation. Endocrinology, 145(3), 1063–1068. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L. J. & Carreiras, M.
(2014) The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: Myth or reality? Experimental Psychology (formerly Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie), 61 (3), 234–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guiney, H. & Machado, L.
(2013) Benefits of regular aerobic exercise for executive functioning in healthy populations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20 (1), 73–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, A. & Valian, V.
(2012) Multi-lingualism and cognitive control: Simon and flanker task performance in monolingual and multilingual young adults. Talk presented at the Psychonomic Society, Minneapolis, November.
Kovács, Á. M., & Mehler, J.
(2009) Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(16), 6556–6560. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., Misra, M., & Guo, T.
(2008) Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica, 128(3), 416–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, P., & Grant, A.
(2015) Identifying the causal link: two approaches toward understanding the relationship between bilingualism and cognitive control. Cortex, 73, 358–360. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, P., Legault, J. & Litcofsky, K. A.
(2014) Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: Anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex, 58, 301–324. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., De Sa, E., & Bialystok, E.
(2011) Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control?. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(04), 588–595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A. & Friedman, N. P.
(2012) The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21 (1), 8–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morales, J., Padilla, F., Gómez-Ariza, C. J., & Bajo, M. T.
(2015) Simultaneous interpretation selectively influences working memory and attentional networks. Acta Psychologica, 155, 82–91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morton, J. B. & Harper, S. N.
(2007) What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10 (6), 719–726. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Onifer, W., & Swinney, D. A.
(1981) Accessing lexical ambiguities during sentence comprehension: Effects of frequency of meaning and contextual bias. Memory & Cognition, 9(3), 225–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R. & Greenberg, Z. I.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66 (2), 232–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O.
(2015) Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poarch, G. J., & Van Hell, J. G.
(2012) Cross-language activation in children’s speech production: Evidence from second language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(3), 419–438. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poeppel, D.
(2012) The maps problem and the mapping problem: two challenges for a cognitive neuroscience of speech and language. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 29 (1–2), 34–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stavrakaki, S., Megari, K., Kosmidis, M. H., Apostolidou, M., & Takou, E.
(2012) Working memory and verbal fluency in simultaneous interpreters. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34(6), 624–633. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Treccani, B., & Mulatti, C.
(2015) No matter who, no matter how… and no matter whether the white matter matters. Why theories of bilingual advantage in executive functioning are so difficult to falsify. Cortex, 73, 349–351. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valian, V.
(2015a) Bilingualism, language, and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 3–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015b) Bilingualism and cognition: A focus on mechanisms. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 47–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
von Bastian, C. C., Souza, A. S., & Gade, M.
(2016) No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(2), 246–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yudes, C., Macizo, P. & Bajo, T.
(2011) The influence of expertise in simultaneous interpreting on non-verbal executive processes. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, Article 309, 9 pages.Google Scholar