Part of
Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond: Questions and insights
Edited by Irina A. Sekerina, Lauren Spradlin and Virginia Valian
[Studies in Bilingualism 57] 2019
► pp. 355369
References
Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. K., Chaudhuri, J. R., & Kaul, S.
(2013) Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 81 (22), 1938–1944. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Allin, S., Masseria, C., & Mossialos, E.
(2009) Measuring socioeconomic differences in use of health care services by wealth versus by income. American Journal of Public Health, 99, 1849–1855. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bak T., & Alladi, S.
(2016) Bilingualism, dementia and the tale of many variables: Why we need to move beyond the Western world. Commentary on Lawton et al. (2015) and Fuller-Thomson (2015) Cortex 74, 315–317. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bak, T. H., Nissan, J. J., Allerhand, M. M., & Deary, I. J.
(2014) Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Annals of Neurology, 75, 959–963. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Freedman, M.
(2007) Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 459–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chertkow, H., Whitehead, V., Phillips, N., Wolfson, C., Atherton, J., & Bergman, H.
(2010) Multilingualism (but not always bilingualism) delays the onset of Alzheimer disease: Evidence from a bilingual community. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 24 (2), 118–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I., Bialystok, E., & Freedman, M.
(2010) Delaying the onset of Alzheimer’s disease: bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology, 75, 1726–1729. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crane, P. K., Gibbons, L. E., Arani, K., Nguyen, V., Rhoads, K., McCurry, S. M., Launer, L., Masaki, K., & White, L.
(2009) Midlife use of written Japanese and protection from late life dementia. Epidemiology, 20, 766–774. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Farias, S. T., Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., Harvey, D., & DeCarli, C.
(2009) Progression of Mild Cognitive Impairment to Dementia in Clinic- vs Community-Based Cohorts. Archives of Neurology, 66 (9), 1151–1157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fratiglioni, L., Paillard-Borg, S., & Winblad, B.
(2004) An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurology, 3 (6), 343–353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuller-Thomson, E.
(2015) Emerging evidence contradicts the hypothesis that bilingualism delays dementia onset. A Commentary on “Age of dementia diagnosis in community dwelling bilingual and monolingual Hispanic Americans” by Lawton et al., 2015. Cortex, 66, 170–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuller-Thomson, E., & Kuh, D.
(2014) The healthy immigrant effect may confound the link between bilingualism and delayed onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex, 52, 128–130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuller-Thomson, E., Nuru-Jeter, A., Richardson, D., Raza, F., & Minkler, M.
(2013) The Hispanic Paradox and older adults’ disabilities: is there a healthy migrant effect?. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 10, 1786–1814. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glymour, M.
(2006) Using Causal Diagrams to Understand Common Problems in Social Epidemiology. In J. Oakes, & J. Kaufman (Eds.). Methods in Social Epidemiology (pp. 393–428). San Francisco: Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Salmon, D. P., Montoya, R. I., & Galasko, D. R.
(2011) Degree of bilingualism predicts age of diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in low-education but not in highly educated Hispanics. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3826–3830. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guzman-Velez, E., & Tranel, D.
(2015) Does Bilingualism contribute to cognitive reserve? Cognitive and neural perspectives. Neuropsychology, 29 (1), 139–150. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hill, T., Angel, J., & Balistreri, K.
(2012) Does the ‘healthy immigrant effect’ extend to cognitive aging? In J. Angel, F. Torres-Gil, & K. Markides (Eds.). Aging, Health, and Longevity in the Mexican-Origin Population (pp. 19–33). New York: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hinton, L., Franz, C. E., Yeo, G., & Levkoff, S. E.
(2005) Conceptions of dementia in a multi-ethnic sample of family caregivers. Journal of American Geriatric Society, 53(8), 1405–1410. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaushanskaya M., & Prior A.
(2015) Variability in the effects of bilingualism on cognition: it is not just about cognition, it is also about bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 27–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kavé, G., Eyal, N., Shorek, A., & Cohen-Mansfield, J.
(2008) Multilingualism and cognitive state in the oldest old. Psychology and Aging, 23, 70–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kormi-Nouri, R., Shojael, R. S., Moniri, S., Gholami, A. R., Akbari-Zardkhaneh, S., & Nilsson, L. G.
(2008) The effect of childhood bilingualism on episodic and semantic memory tasks. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49, 93–109. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kovács, Á.M., & Mehler, J.
(2009) Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 106, 6556–6560. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lawton, D. M., Gasquoine, P. G., & Weimer, A. A.
(2015) Age of dementia diagnosis in community dwelling bilingual and monolingual Hispanic Americans. Cortex, 66, 141–145. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loizou, M., & Stuart, M.
(2003) Phonological awareness in monolingual and bilingual English and Greek five-year-olds. Journal of Research on Reading, 26, 3–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology (Hove, England), 25 (5), 605–621. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Grady, C. L.
(2011) Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 16808–16813. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Manly, J. J., Jacobs, D. M., Touradji, P., Small, S. A., & Stern, Y.
(2002) Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 341–348. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O.
(2016) Should the search for bilingual advantages in executive function continue? Cortex, 74, 305–314. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prior, A. & Gollan, T. H.
(2011) Good Language-Switchers are Good Task-Switchers: Evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English Bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 682–691. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sachdev, P. & Valenzuela, M.
(2009) Brain and cognitive reserve. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17 (3): 175–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanders, A. E., Hall, C. B., Katz, M. J., & Lipton, R. B.
(2012) Non-native language use and risk of incident dementia in the elderly. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 29, 99–108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scaltritti, M., Peressotti, F., & Miozzo, M.
(2015) Bilingual advantage and language switch: What’s the linkage?. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schweizer, T. A., Ware, J., Fischer, C. E., Craik, F. I., & Bialystok, E.
(2012) Bilingualism as a contributor to cognitive reserve: evidence from brain atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex, 48 (8), 991–996. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stern, Y.
(2002) What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448–460. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurology, 11 (11), 1006–1012. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tao, L., Taft, M., & Gollan, T.
(2015) The Bilingual Switching Advantage: Sometimes Related to Bilingual Proficiency, Sometimes Not. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 21, 531–544. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valian, V.
(2014) Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 3–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yeung, C. M., St. John, P. D., Menec, V., & Tyas, S. L.
(2014) Is bilingualism associated with a lower risk of dementia in community-living older adults? Cross-sectional and prospective analyses. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 28, 326–332. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yow, W. Q., & Li, X.
(2015) Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zahodne, L. B., Schofield, P. W., Farrell, M., Stern, Y., & Manly, J. J.
(2014) Bilingualism does not alter age-related cognitive decline or dementia risk among Spanish-speaking immigrants. Neuropsychology, 28, 238–246. DOI logoGoogle Scholar