Part of
L3 Development After the Initial State
Edited by Megan M. Brown-Bousfield, Suzanne Flynn and Éva Fernández-Berkes
[Studies in Bilingualism 65] 2023
► pp. 121149
References (88)
References
Bardel, C., & Falk, Y. (2007). The role of the second language in third language acquisition: The case of Germanic syntax. Second Language Research, 23, 459–484. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). The L2 status factor and the declarative/procedural distinction. In J. Cabrelli, S. Flynn, & J. Rothman (Eds.), Third language acquisition in adulthood (pp. 61–78). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2018). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 6.0.37). University of Amsterdam. Retrieved on 5 June 2023 from [URL]
Bravo-Díaz, C. (2020, December). The acquisition of stop consonants by Chinese learners of Spanish: A cross-sectional study. Presented at the Hispanic and Lusophone Symposium.
Brown-Bousfield, M. M., & Chang, C. B. (2023). Regressive cross-linguistic influence in multilingual speech rhythm: The role of language similarity. In M. Brown-Bousfield, É. Fernández-Berkes, & S. Flynn (Eds.), L3 development after the initial state (pp. 49–71). John Benjamins. (this volume). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J., & Perpiñán, S. (2014). Subject pronouns and clitics in the Spanish interlanguage of French L1 speakers. In L. Teddiman (Ed.), Proceedings of the 2014 annual conference of the Canadian Linguistic Association. Retrieved on 5 June 2023 from [URL]
Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2017). Testing the phonological permeability hypothesis: L3 phonological effects on L1 versus L2 systems. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 698–717. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J., & Rothman, J. (2010). On L3 acquisition and phonological permeability: A new test case for debates on the mental representation of non-native phonological systems. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 48(2), 275–296.Google Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J., & Wrembel, M. (2016). Investigating the acquisition of phonology in a third language – A state of the science and an outlook for the future. International Journal of Multilingualism, 13(4), 395–409. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carrasco, P., Hualde, J. I., & Simonet, M. (2012). Dialectal differences in Spanish voiced obstruent allophony: Costa Rican versus Iberian Spanish. Phonetica, 69(3), 149–179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chang, C. B. (2015). Determining cross-linguistic phonological similarity between segments: The primacy of abstract aspects of similarity. In G. B. Chang, The segment in phonetics and phonology (pp. 199–217). John Wiley & Sons. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chao, K. Y., & Chen, L. M. (2008). A cross-linguistic study of voice onset time in stop consonant productions. International Journal of Computational Linguistics & Chinese Language Processing, 13(2), 215–232.Google Scholar
Chen, Y. (2007). A comparison of Spanish produced by Chinese L2 learners and native speakers – An acoustic phonetics approach (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. Retrieved on 5 Jube 2023 from [URL]
Chen, L. M., Chao, K. Y., & Peng, J. F. (2007). VOT productions of word-initial stops in Mandarin and English: A cross-language study. In K.-H. Chen & B. Chen (Eds.), ROCLING 2007 Poster Papers (pp. 303–317). ACLCLP.Google Scholar
De Angelis, G. (2007). Third or additional language acquisition. Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Bot, K. & Larsen-Freeman, D. (2011). Researching second language development from a dynamic systems theory perspective. In M. H. Verspoor, K. de Bot, & W. Lowie (Eds.), A dynamic approach to second language development. Methods and techniques (pp. 5–23). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Bot, K., Lowie, W., & Verspoor, M. (2007). A dynamic systems theory approach to second language acquisition. Bilingualism, 10(1), 7–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duanmu, S. (2007). The phonology of standard Chinese. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Falk, Y., & Bardel, C. (2010). The study of role of the background languages in third language acquisition. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL) 48, 185–220.Google Scholar
Falk, Y., Lindqvist, C., & Bardel, C. (2015). The role of L1 explicit metalinguistic knowledge in L3 oral production at the initial state. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 18(2), 227–235. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernández-Berkes, É., & Flynn, S. (2021). Vindicating the need for a principled theory of language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11(1), 30–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2023). Grammatical mapping in L3 acquisition: A theory of development. In M. Brown-Bousfield, É. Fernández-Berkes, & S. Flynn (Eds.), L3 development after the initial state (pp. 8–28). John Benjamins. (this volume). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1991). Age of learning affects the authenticity of voice-onset time (VOT) in stop consonants produced in a second language. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89, 395–411. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995). Second language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 233–277). York Press.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Bohn, O-. S. (2021). The revised Speech Learning Model (SLM-r). In R. Wayland (Ed.), Second language speech learning: Theoretical and empirical progress (pp. 3–83). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Eefting, W. (1986). Linguistic and developmental effects on the production and perception of stop consonants. Phonetica, 43, 155–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1987). Production and perception of English stops by native Spanish speakers. Journal of Phonetics, 15, 67–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flynn, S., Foley, C., & Vinnitskaya, I. (2004). The cumulative-enhancement model for language acquisition: Comparing adults’ and children’s patterns of development in first, second and third language acquisition of relative clauses. International Journal of Multilingualism, 1(1), 3–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallardo, M., & Montrul, S. (2023). Property-by-property transfer in L3 Italian. In M. Brown-Bousfield, É. Fernández-Berkes, & S. Flynn (Eds.), L3 development after the initial state (pp. 236–271). John Benjamins. (this volume). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gardner, R. C. (2006). The socio-educational model of second language acquisition: A research paradigm. Eurosla Yearbook, 6, 237–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
González, A. J., & Rothman, J. (2017). Coming of age in L3 initial stages transfer models: Deriving developmental predictions and looking towards the future. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 683–697. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gut, U. (2010). Cross-linguistic influence in L3 phonological acquisition. International Journal of Multilingualism, 7(1), 19–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hammarberg, B. (2018). L3, the tertiary language. In A. Bonnet & P. Siemund (Eds.), Foreign language education in multilingual classrooms (pp. 127–150). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hammarberg, B., & Williams, S. (1993). A study of third language acquisition. In B. Hammarberg (Eds.), Problem, process, product in language learning (pp. 60–70). Stockholm University.Google Scholar
Herd, W. (2020). Sociophonetic voice onset time variation in Mississippi English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 147(1), 596–605. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hermas, A. (2010). Language acquisition as computational resetting: Verb movement in L3 initial state. International Journal of Multilingualism, 7, 202–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015). The categorization of the relative complementizer phrase in third-language English: A feature re-assembly account. International Journal of Bilingualism 19, 587–607. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hualde, J. I. (2005). The sounds of Spanish. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hunnicutt, L., & Morris, P. A. (2016). Prevoicing and aspiration in Southern American English. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 22(1), 215–224.Google Scholar
Keating, P. A. (1984). Phonetic and phonological representation of stop consonant voicing. Language, 60(2), 286. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kong, S. (2015). L3 initial state: Typological primacy driven, L2 factor determined, or L1 feature oriented? Taiwan Journal of Linguistics, 13(2), 79–116.Google Scholar
Kopečková, R. (2015). Differences in the perception of English vowel sounds by child L2 and L3 learners. In U. Gut, R. Fuchs, E.-M. Wunder (Eds.), Universal or diverse paths to English phonology (pp. 71–89). Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). The bilingual advantage in L3 learning: A developmental study of rhotic sounds. International Journal of Multilingualism, 13(4), 410–425. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kopečková, R., Marecka, M., Wrembel, M., & Gut, U. (2016). Interactions between three phonological subsystems of young multilinguals: The influence of language status. International Journal of Multilingualism, 13(4), 426–443. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krashen, S. (1981). Second language acquisition. Second Language Learning, 3(7), 19–39.Google Scholar
Li, P., Zhang, F., Yu, A., & Zhao, X. (2020). Language History Questionnaire (LHQ3): An enhanced tool for assessing multilingual experience. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(5), 938–944. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liao, S. J. (2005). Interlanguage production of English stop consonants: A VOT analysis (Unpublished master’s thesis). National Kaohsiung Normal University.
Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20(3), 384–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liu, Z. (2016). Exploring cross-linguistic influence: Perception and production of L1, L2 and L3 bilabial stops by Mandarin Chinese speakers (Unpublished master’s thesis). Universitat Autónoma de Barcelona. Retrieved on 5 June 2023 from [URL]
Llama, R., & Cardoso, W. (2018). Revisiting (non-) native influence in VOT production: Insights from advanced L3 Spanish. Languages, 3(3), 30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Llama, R., Cardoso, W., & Collins, L. (2010). The influence of language distance and language status on the acquisition of L3 phonology. International Journal of Multilingualism, 7(1), 39–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lowie, W. M., van Dijk, M., Chan, H. P., & Verspoor, M. H. (2017). Finding the key to successful L2 learning in groups and individuals. Journal of Language Teaching and Learning, 7, 127–148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lowie, W. M., & Verspoor, M. H. (2019). Individual differences and the ergodicity problem. Language Learning, 69, 184–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Major, R. C. (1987). English voiceless stop production by speakers of Brazilian Portuguese. Journal of Phonetics, 15, 197–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meara, P. M., & Rogers, V. E. (2019). The LLAMA tests v3. Lognostics. Retrieved on 5 June 2023 from [URL]
Nagle, C. L. (2017). Individual developmental trajectories in the L2 acquisition of Spanish spirantization. Journal of Second Language Pronunciation, 3(2), 218–241. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019). A longitudinal study of voice onset time development in L2 Spanish stops. Applied Linguistics, 40(1), 86–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nagle, C. L., Morales-Front, A., Moorman, C., & Sanz, C. (2016). Disentangling research on study abroad and pronunciation: Methodological and programmatic considerations. In D. Velliaris & D. Coleman-George (Ed.), Handbook of research on study abroad programs and outbound mobility (pp. 673–695). IGI Global. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Onishi, H. (2016). The effects of L2 experience on L3 perception. International Journal of Multilingualism, 13(4), 459–475. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ortega, L., Iwashita, N., Norris, J. M., & Rabie, S. (2002, October). An investigation of elicited imitation tasks in crosslinguistic SLA research. Presented at Second Language Research Forum, Toronto.
Patience, M. (2018). Acquisition of the tap-trill contrast by L1 Mandarin–L2 English–L3 Spanish speakers. Languages, 3(4), 42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peirce, J., Gray, J., Halchenko, Y., Britton, D., Rokem, A., & Strangman, G. (2011). PsychoPy–A psychology software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1–2), 8–13. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peng, J. F., Chen, L. M., & Lin, Y. Y. (2009). Tonal effects on voice onset time: Stops in Mandarin and Hakka. In 21st Conference on Computational Linguistics and Speech Processing, ROCLING 2009 (pp. 115–123). ACLCLP.Google Scholar
Puig-Mayenco, E., González Alonso, J., & Rothman, J. (2020). A systematic review of transfer studies in third language acquisition. Second Language Research, 36(1), 31–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pyun, K-S. (2005). A model of interlanguage analysis – The case of Swedish by Korean speakers. In B. Hufeisen & R. Fouser (Eds.), Introductory readings in L3 (pp. 55–70). Stauffenberg.Google Scholar
Riney, T. J., & Takagi, N. (1999). Global foreign accent and voice onset time among Japanese EFL speakers. Language Learning, 49(2), 275–302. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rochet, B. L., & Fei, Y. (1991). Effect of consonant and vowel context on Mandarin Chinese VOT: Production and perception. Canadian Acoustics, 19(4), 105–106.Google Scholar
Rosner, B. S., López-Bascuas, L. E., Garcı́a-Albea, J. E., & Fahey, R. P. (2000). Voice-onset times for Castilian Spanish initial stops. Journal of Phonetics, 28(2), 217–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J. (2011). L3 syntactic transfer selectivity and typological determinacy: The Typological Primacy Model. Second Language Research, 27(1), 107–127. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015). Linguistic and cognitive motivations for the Typological Primacy Model (TPM) of third language (L3) transfer: Timing of acquisition and proficiency considered. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 179–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J., & Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2010). What variables condition syntactic transfer? A look at the L3 initial state. Second Language Research, 26(2), 189–218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanz, C., & Lado, B. (2008). Third language acquisition research methods. In N. Hornberger (Ed.), Encyclopedia of language and education (Vol. 10, pp. 113–135). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanz, C., Park, H. I., & Lado, B. (2015). A functional approach to cross-linguistic influence in ab initio L3 acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 236–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simonet, M., Hualde, J. I., & Nadeu, M. (2012). Lenition of/d/in spontaneous Spanish and Catalan. In Thirteenth Annual Conference of the International Speech Communication Association (Interspeech 2012). ISCA. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slabakova, R. (2017). The scalpel model of third language acquisition. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 651–665. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2023). A shared linguistic system of multilingual representations. In M. Brown-Bousfield, É. Fernández-Berkes, & S. Flynn (Eds.), L3 development after the initial state (pp. 29–48). John Benjamins. (this volume). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tremblay, M. (2007, August). L2 influence on L3 pronunciation: Native-like VOT in the L3 Japanese of English-French bilinguals. Paper presented at the L3 Phonology Satellite Workshop of ICPhS XVI, Freiburg, Germany.
Westergaard, M., Mitrofanova, N., Mykhaylyk, R., & Rodina, Y. (2017). Crosslinguistic influence in the acquisition of a third language: The Linguistic Proximity Model. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 666–682. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wrembel, M. (2010). L2-accented speech in L3 production. International Journal of Multilingualism, 7(1), 75–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011). Cross-linguistic influence in third language acquisition of voice onset time. In L. Wai-Sum & E. Zee (Eds.), Proceedings of the 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences ICPhS (pp. 2157–2160). City University of Hong Kong.Google Scholar
(2014). VOT patterns in the acquisition of third language phonology. Concordia Papers in Applied Linguistics (COPAL), 5, 751–771.Google Scholar
(2021). Multilingual acquisition property by property: A view from a wider perspective. Second Language Research, 37(3), 441–447. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wrembel, M., Marecka, M., & Kopečková, R. (2019). Extending perceptual assimilation model to L3 phonological acquisition. International Journal of Multilingualism, 16(4), 513–533. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wunder, E. M. (2010). Phonological cross-linguistic influence in third or additional language acquisition. In K. Dziubalska-Kołaczyk, M. Wrembel, & M. Kul (Eds.), New sounds 2010: Proceedings of the 6th International Symposium on the Acquisition of Second Language Speech (pp. 566–571). Adam Mickiewicz University.Google Scholar
Yang, Y., X. Chen & Q. Xiao. (2020). Cross-similarity in L2 speech learning: Evidence from the acquisition of Russian stop contrasts by Mandarin speakers. Second Language Research 38, 3–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zampini, M. L. (1998). The relationship between the production and perception of L2 Spanish stops. Texas Papers in Foreign Language Education, 3(3), 85–100.Google Scholar
(2013). Voice onset time in second language Spanish. In K. L. Geeslin (Eds.), The handbook of Spanish second language acquisition (pp.111–129). Wiley Online Library. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhang, L. (2022). ¿Pollo o bollo? Acquisition of voicing contrast by Chinese learners of L3 Spanish. Hispania, 105(4), 595–613. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (2)

Cited by two other publications

Yang, Yufeng, Fabian Santiago, F. Neveu, S. Prévost, A. Montébran, A. Steuckardt, G. Bergounioux, G. Merminod & G. Philippe
2024. L’influence translinguistique dans la perception des consonnes occlusives en français L3 par des apprenants sinophones : une étude exploratoire. SHS Web of Conferences 191  pp. 09002 ff. DOI logo
Brown-Bousfield, Megan M. & Charles B. Chang
2023. Regressive cross-linguistic influence in multilingual speech rhythm. In L3 Development After the Initial State [Studies in Bilingualism, 65],  pp. 49 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.