Part of
L3 Development After the Initial State
Edited by Megan M. Brown-Bousfield, Suzanne Flynn and Éva Fernández-Berkes
[Studies in Bilingualism 65] 2023
► pp. 205235
References (83)
References
Alonso, J. G., Banón, J. A., DeLuca, V., Miller, D., Soares, S. M. P., Puig-Mayenco, E., Slaats, S., & Rothman, J. (2020). Event related potentials at initial exposure in third language acquisition: Implications from an artificial mini-grammar study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56, 100939. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bardel, C., & Falk, Y. (2007). The role of the second language in third language acquisition: The case of Germanic syntax. Second Language Research, 23(4), 459–484. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). The L2 status factor and the declarative/procedural. In J. Cabrelli, S. Flynn & Rothman (Eds.), Third language acquisition in adulthood (pp. 61–78). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bel, A., Sagarra, N., Comínguez, J., & García-Alcaraz, E. (2016). Transfer and proficiency effects in L2 processing of subject anaphora. Lingua, 184, 134–159. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berkes, É., & Flynn, S. (2012). Enhanced L3…Ln acquisition and its implications for language teaching. In D. Gabrys-Barker (Ed.), Cross-linguistic influences in multilingual language acquisition (pp. 1–22). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kucˇera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 977–990. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Çabuk-Ballı, S. (2021). Cross-linguistic influence in the acquisition of adpositions in L3. International Journal of Multilingualism, 1–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, B., Gelman, A., Hoffman, M. D., Lee, D., Goodrich, B., Betancourt, M., … & Riddell, A. (2017). Stan: A probabilistic programming language. Journal of statistical software, 76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coughlin, C. E., & Tremblay, A. (2013). Proficiency and working memory-based explanations for nonnative speakers’ sensitivity to agreement in sentence processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34(3), 615–646. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dave, A. (2004). Oxford placement test 2: Test pack. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2017). Knowledge and skill in SLA. In S. Loewen & M. Sato (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of instructed second language acquisition (pp. 15–32). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drummond, A. (2013). Ibex farm. Retrieved on 6 June 2023 from [URL]
Edmonds, C. J. (1955). Prepositions and personal affixes in southern Kurdish. Bulletin of the School of Oriental and African Studies, University of London, 17(3), 490–502. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2008). Implicit and explicit knowledge about language. In N. H. Hornberger (Ed.), Encyclopedia of language and education (Vol. 6, 1–13). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Sagarra, N. (2011). Learned attention in adult language acquisition: A replication and generalization study and meta-analysis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33(4), 589–624. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, V., & Tyler, A. (2005). Applying cognitive linguistics to pedagogical grammar: The English prepositions of verticality. Revista Brasileira de Linguistica Aplicada, 5(2), 11–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernández-Berkes, É., & Flynn, S. (2021). Vindicating the need for a principled theory of language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11(1), 30–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2023). Grammatical mapping in L3 acquisition: A theory of development. In M. M. Brown, S. Flynn, & É. Fernández-Berkes (Eds.), L3 development after the initial state (pp. 8–28). John Benjamins. (this volume). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flynn, S., Foley, C., & Vinnitskaya, I. (2004). The cumulative-enhancement model for language acquisition: Comparing adults’ and children’s patterns of development in first, second and third language acquisition of relative clauses. International Journal of Multilingualism, 1(1), 3–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D. (1995). The time course of syntactic activation during language processing: A model based on neuropsychological and neurophysiological data. Brain and Language, 49, 259–281. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garcia Mayo, M. del P., & Slabakova, R. (2015). Object drop in L3 acquisition. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19(5), 483–498. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gelman, A., Carlin, J. B., Stern, H. S., & Rubin, D. B. (2014). Bayesian data analysis (Vol. 2). Chapman.Google Scholar
Hagège, C. (2010). Adpositions. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hagoort, P. (2003). How the brain solves the binding problem for language: A neurocomputational model of syntactic processing. Neuro-Image, 20, 18–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hahne, A., Mueller, J. L., & Clahsen, H. (2006). Morphological processing in a second language: Behavioral and event-related brain potential evidence for storage and decomposition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(1),121–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Havik, E., Roberts, L., van Hout, R., Schreuder, R., & Haverkort, M. (2009). Processing subject-object ambiguities in the L2: A self-paced reading study with German L2 learners of Dutch. Language Learning, 59(1), 73–112. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hermas, A. (2015). The categorization of the relative complementizer phrase in third-language English: A feature re-assembly account. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19(5), 587–607. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H. (2010). Ultimate attainment in L2 inflection: Performance similarities between non-native and native speakers. Lingua, 120(4), 901–931. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019). Cross-linguistic influence in the child third language acquisition of grammar: Sentence comprehension and production among Turkish-German and German learners of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 23(2), 567–583. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jach, D. (2018). A usage-based approach to preposition placement in English as a second language. Language Learning, 68(1), 271–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, C. (2008). Proficiency level and the interaction of lexical and morphosyntactic information during L2 sentence processing. Language Learning, 58(4), 875–909. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, C., & Dussias, P. (2009). Cross-linguistic differences and their impact on L2 sentence processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 65–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, C., & van Hell, J. (2011). The effects of L2 proficiency level on the processing of wh-questions among Dutch second language speakers of English. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 49, 195–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jarvis, S., & Pavlenko, A. (2008). Crosslinguistic influence in language and cognition. Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jegerski, J. (2014). Self-paced reading. Research methods in second language psycholinguistics (pp. 20–49). Routledge.Google Scholar
Jegerski, J., Keating, G. D., & VanPatten, B. (2016). On-line relative clause attachment strategy in heritage speakers of Spanish. International Journal of Bilingualism, 20(3), 254–268. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N. (2004). Morphological insensitivity in second language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25(4), 603. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007). Selective integration of linguistic knowledge in adult second language learning. Language Learning, 57, 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N., Novokshanova, E., Masuda, K., & Wang, X. (2011). Morphological congruency and the acquisition of L2 morphemes. Language Learning, 61, 940–967. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A. (2005). The influence of first language on the processing of wh-movement in English as a second language. Second Language Research, 21, 121–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Woolley, J. D. (1982). Paradigms and processes in reading comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111(2), 228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keating, G. D., & Jegerski, J. (2015). Experimental designs in sentence processing research: A methodological review and user’s guide. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37(1), 1–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kellerman, E. (1978). Giving learners a break: Native language intuitions as a source of predictions about transferability. Working Papers on Bilingualism Toronto, 1(15), 59–92.Google Scholar
Klein, E. C. (1995). Second versus third language acquisition: Is there a difference? Language learning, 45(3), 419–466. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kurzon, D., & Adler, S. (2008). Adpositions: Pragmatic, semantic and syntactic perspectives. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, E., Lu, D., & Garnsey, S. (2013). L1 word order and sensitivity to verb bias in L2 processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 761–775. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leung, Y.-k. I. (2003). Failed features versus full transfer full access in the acquisition of a third language: Evidence from tense and agreement. In J. M. Liceras, H. Zobl, & H. Goodluck (Eds.), Proceedings of the 6th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference (pp. 199–207). Cascadsilla Press.Google Scholar
Libert, A. R. (2008). Case marking of Turkic adpositional objects. In D. Kurzon & S. Adler (Eds.), Adpositions: Pragmatic, semantic and syntactic perspectives (pp. 229–255). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lowie, W., & Verspoor, M. (2004). Input versus transfer? The role of frequency and similarity in the acquisition of L2 prepositions. In M. Achard & S. Niemeier (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics, second language acquisition, and foreign language teaching (pp. 77–94). De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacKenzie, D. (1961). Kurdish dialect studies (Vol I). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1997). Implicit and explicit processes: Commentary. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19(2), 277–281. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005). Extending the competition model. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9(1), 69–84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marinis, T. (2010). Using on-line processing methods in language acquisition research. In E. Blom & S. Unsworth (Eds.), Experimental methods in language acquisition research (pp. 139–162). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marinis, T., Roberts, L., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H. (2005). Gaps in second language processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 53–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marsden, E., Thompson, S., & Plonsky, L. (2018). A methodological synthesis of self-paced reading in second language research. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39, 861–904. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Dias, R., & Santos, H. (2011). Clitics and object expression in the L3 acquisition of Brazilian Portuguese: Structural similarity matters for transfer. Second Language Research, 27(1), 21–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ó Laoire, M. (2005). L3 in Ireland: A preliminary study of learners’ metalinguistic awareness. In R. J. Fouser & B. Hufeisen (Eds.), Introductory readings in L3 (pp. 47–55). Tübingen.Google Scholar
Papadopoulou, D., & Clahsen, H. (2003). Parsing strategies in L1 and L2 sentence processing: A study of relative clause attachment in Greek. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 501–528. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perpiñán, S. (2014). L2 grammar and L2 processing in the acquisition of Spanish prepositional relative clauses. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 577–596. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Puig-Mayenco, E., González Alonso, J., & Rothman, J. (2020). A systematic review of transfer studies in third language acquisition. Second Language Research, 36(1), 31–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rah, A. (2010). Transfer in L3 sentence processing: Evidence from relative clause attachment ambiguities. International Journal of Multilingualism, 7(2), 147–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberts, L., & Liszka, S. A. (2013). Processing tense/aspect-agreement violations on-line in the second language: A self-paced reading study with French and German L2 learners of English. Second Language Research, 29(4), 413–439. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberts, L., Mackey, A., & Marsden, E. (2016). Self-paced reading and L2 grammatical processing. In A. Mackey & E. Marsden (Eds.), Advancing methodology and practice. The IRIS repository of instruments for research into second languages (pp. 58–72). Routledge.Google Scholar
Rossi, S., Gugler, M. F., Friederici, A. D., & Hahne, A. (2006). The impact of proficiency on syntactic second-language processing of German and Italian: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(12), 2030–2048. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J. (2015). Linguistic and cognitive motivations for the typological primacy model (TPM) of third language (L3) transfer: Timing of acquisition and proficiency considered. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2),179–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J., & Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2010). What variables condition syntactic transfer? A look at the L3 initial state. Second Language Research, 26(2), 189–218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J., & Pereira Soares, S. M. (2020). Cognitive states in third language acquisition and beyond: Theoretical and methodological paths forward. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11(1), 89–95.Google Scholar
Sabourin, L., & Stowe, L. A. (2008). Second language processing: When are first and second languages processed similarly? Second Language Research, 24(3), 397–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sagarra, N., & Herschensohn, J. (2011). Proficiency and animacy effects on L2 gender agreement processes during comprehension. Language Learning, 61, 80–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slabakova, R. (2017). The scalpel model of third language acquisition. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 651–665. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sokolova, M., & Slabakova, R. (2019). L3 sentence processing: Language-specific or phenomenon-sensitive? Languages, 4(3), 54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Song, Y. (2015). L2 processing of plural inflection in English. Language Learning, 65, 233–267. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tokowicz, N., & MacWhinney, B. (2005). Implicit and explicit measures of sensitivity to violations in second language grammar: An event-related potential investigation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2), 173–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tokowicz, N., & Warren, T. (2010). Beginning adult L2 learners’ sensitivity to morphosyntactic violations: A self-paced reading study. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(7), 1092–1106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tucker, M. A., Idrissi, A., & Almeida, D. (2015). Representing number in the real-time processing of agreement: Self-paced reading evidence from Arabic. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Buren, P., & Smith, M. S. (1985). The acquisition of preposition stranding by second language learners and parametric variation. Interlanguage Studies Bulletin (Utrecht), 1(1), 18–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Hell, J. G., & Tokowicz, N. (2010). Event-related brain potentials and second language learning: Syntactic processing in late L2 learners at different L2 proficiency levels. Second Language Research, 26(1), 43–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Heuven, W. J., Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2014). Subtlex-UK: A new and improved word frequency database for British English. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(6), 1176–1190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B., Keating, G. D., & Leeser, M. J. (2012). Missing verbal inflections as a representational problem: Evidence from self-paced reading. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 2(2), 109–140. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Westergaard, M. (2021). L3 acquisition and crosslinguistic influence as co-activation: Response to commentaries on the keynote ‘microvariation in multilingual situations: The importance of property-by-property acquisition’. Second Language Research, 37(3), 501–518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Westergaard, M., Mitrofanova, N., Mykhaylyk, R., & Rodina, Y. (2017). Crosslinguistic influence in the acquisition of a third language: The linguistic proximity model. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 666–682. DOI logoGoogle Scholar