Chapter 1
The developing language foundation for reading comprehension
Vocabulary, complex syntax and extended discourse from preschool to grade one
Oral language is a strong predictor of reading, but few studies have described the development of multiple strands of language ability from preschool through grade one. The development of vocabulary has been thoroughly examined, but few large studies have examined the emergence and interrelationships among vocabulary, syntax and extended discourse between preschool and grade one. This study describes the development of these three strands of language ability and changing patterns of interrelationships among them, with data from 356 African-American children from low-income homes in the United States. Data were collected at the beginning and end of preschool and the end of kindergarten and first grade. Constructs for vocabulary and extended discourse were closely related in preschool and kindergarten, with syntax being less related. By grade one all three constructs were more closely associated. Our results indicate that syntax may draw on linguistic and cognitive capacities that are different from vocabulary and extended discourse, but over time all language capabilities become more closely associated and mutually reinforcing. These findings further support the importance of educational approaches that foster all dimensions of language.
Article outline
- Introduction
- Background
- Language and reading comprehension
- Vocabulary
- Syntax
- Narrative, extended discourse and reading
- Current study
- Data source
- Sample selection
- Assessments
- Development of language strands from pre-k to end-of-grade one
- Strength of our constructs
- Longitudinal performance
- Dimensionality of language
- Early language and literacy development
- Implications for practice
- Acknowledgements
-
Note
-
References
References (93)
References
Arunachalam, S., & Waxman, S. R. (2010). Meaning from syntax: Evidence from 2-year-olds. Cognition, 114(3), 442–446. 

Bamberg, M. (1997). A constructivist approach to narrative development. In M. Bamberg (Ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches (pp.89–132). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bishop, D., & Edmundson, A. (1987). Language-impaired 4-year-olds. Distinguishing transient from persistent impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52(2), 156–173. 

Booth, A. E., & Waxman, S. R. (2002). Word learning is ‘smart’: Evidence that conceptual information affects preschoolers' extension of novel words. Cognition, 84(1), B11–B22. 

Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (1996). The nature of the relationship between comprehension skill and the ability to tell a story. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 187–201. 

Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2006). Profiles of children with specific reading comprehension difficulties. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 683–696. 

Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V., Barnes, M. A., & Bryant, P. E. (2001). Comprehension skill, inference-making ability, and their relation to knowledge. Memory & Cognition, 29(6), 850–859. 

Catts, H. W., Adolf, S. M., & Weismer, S. E. (2006). Language deficits Ωin poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of reading. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(2), 278 – 293. 

Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(6), 1142–1157. 

Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 331–361. 

Champion, T., Hyter, Y. D., McCabe, A., & Bland-Stewart, L. M. (2003). “A matter of vocabulary”: Performances of low-income African-American Head Start children on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary test-III. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 24(3), 121–127. 

Champion, T. B., & McCabe, A. (2015). Narrative structures of African American children. In The Oxford handbook of African American language. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Clarke, P. J., Snowling, M. J., Truelove, E., & Hulme, C. (2010). Ameliorating children's reading-comprehension difficulties: A randomized controlled trial. Psychological Science, 21(8), 1106–1116. 

Cooper, D. H., Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., & Schatschneider, C. (2002). The contribution of oral language skills to the development of phonological awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23(3), 399–416. 

Craig, H. K., Connor, C. M., & Washington, J. A. (2003). Early positive predictors of later reading comprehension for African American students: A preliminary investigation. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 34(1), 31–43. 

Craig, H. K., & Washington, J. A. (2006). Language and literacy skills of African-American students. In D. K. Dickinson, & S. B. Neuman (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research, Vol. 2 (pp.132–148.). New York, NY: Guilford.
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). Early reading acquisition and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 years later. Developmental Psychology, 33(6), 934–945. 

Dickinson, D. K. (2011). Teachers' language practices and academic outcomes of preschool children. Science, 333(6045), 964–967. 

Dickinson, D. K., & McCabe, A. (1991). A social interactionist account of language and literacy development. In J. Kavanaugh (Ed.), The language continuum (pp.1–40). Parkton, MD: The York Press.
Dickinson, D. K., & McCabe, A. (2001). Bringing it all together: The multiple origins, skills and environmental supports of early literacy. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 16(4), 186–202. 

Dickinson, D. K., McCabe, A., Anastasopoulos, L., Peisner-Feinberg, E., & Poe, M. D. (2003). The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The interrelationships among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print knowledge among preschool-aged children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 465–481. 

Dickinson, D. K., & Porche, M. V. (2011). Relation between language experiences in preschool classrooms and children's kindergarten and fourth-grade language and reading abilities. Child Development, 82(3), 870–886. 

Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P. O. (Eds.). (2001). Beginning literacy with language: Young children learning at home and school. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary test (4th ed.). Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson.
Fazio, B. B., Naremore, R. C., & Connell, P. J. (1996). Tracking children from poverty at risk for specific language impairment. A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39(3), 611–624. 

Feagans, L., & Appelbaum, M. I. (1986). Validation of language subtypes in learning disabled children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78(5), 358. 

Ferguson, B., Graf, E., & Waxman, S. R. (2014). Infants use known verbs to learn novel nouns: Evidence from 15- and 19-month-olds. Cognition, 131(1), 139–146. 

Garcia, R. J., & Cain, K. (2014). Decoding and reading comprehension: A meta-analysis to identify which reader and assessment characteristics influence the strength of the relationship in English. Review of Educational Research, 84(1), 74–111. 

Gee, J. P. (2008). Social linguistics and literacies: Ideology in discourses (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.
Glasgow, C., & Cowley, J. (1994). Renfrew Bus Story test (North American Edition). Centreville, DE: Centreville School.
Gordon, P. (1988). Count/mass category acquisition: Distributional distinctions in children's speech. Journal of Child Language, 15(1), 109–128. 

Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday lives of American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.
Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. (1996). The origins of grammar: Evidence from early language comprehension. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2(2), 127–160. 

Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2007). Spoken word recognition by Latino children learning Spanish as their first language. Journal of Child Language, 34(2), 227–249. 

Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11(6), F31–F39. 

Juel, C., Griffith, P. L., & Gough, P. B. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78(4), 243–255. 

Kaiser, A. P., Dickinson, D. K., Hofer, K. G., Roberts, M., Darrow, C. L., McCleod, R., & Freiberg, J. B. (2010, June). The effects of two language-focused preschool curricula on children's achievement in preschool and kindergarten. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Educational Effectiveness, Washington D.C.
Kendeou, P., Savage, R., & van den Broek, P. (2009a). Revisiting the simple view of reading. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 353–370. 

Kendeou, P., White, M. J., van den Broek, P., & Lynch, J. S. (2009b). Predicting reading comprehension in early elementary school: The independent contributions of oral language and decoding skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 765–778. 

Kim, Y. S. G. (2015). Language and cognitive predictors of text comprehension: Evidence from multivariate analysis. Child Development, 86(1), 128–144. 

Kim, Y. S. G. (2016). Direct and mediated effects of language and cognitive skills on comprehension of oral narrative texts (listening comprehension) for children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 101–120. 

Kintsch, W. (2005). An overview of top-down and bottom-up effects in comprehension: The CI perspective. Discourse Processes, 39(2–3), 125–128. 

Lawrence, J. F., Capotosto, L., Branum-Martin, L., White, C., & Snow, C. E. (2012). Language proficiency, home-language status, and English vocabulary development: A longitudinal follow-up of the Word Generation program. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(3), 437–451. 

Lesaux, N. K., Kieffer, M. J., Faller, S. E., & Kelley, J. G. (2010). The effectiveness and ease of implementation of an academic vocabulary intervention for linguistically diverse students in urban middle schools. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(2), 196–228. 

Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Anthony, J. L. (2000). Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596–613. 

Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11(3), F9–F16. 

McCabe, A., & Bliss, L. S. (2003). Patterns of narrative discourse: A multicultural lifespan approach. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
McCabe, A., & Champion, T. B. (2010). A matter of vocabulary II: Low-income African-American children's performance on the Expressive Vocabulary test. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 31(3), 162–169. 

McLeod, R. H. (2010). The relationship between teacher language use in enhanced milieu teaching sessions an child language outcomes. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Vanderbilt University.
Menyuk, P., Chesnick, M., Liebergott, J. W., Korngold, B., D'Agostino, R., & Belanger, A. (1991). Predicting reading problems in at-risk children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 34(4), 893–903. 

Michaels, S. (1981). Sharing time: Children's narrative styles and differential access to literacy. Language and Society, 10, 423–441. 

Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40(5), 665–681. 

Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27(4), 342–356. 

National Center for Educational Sciences. (2013). The nation's report card: Trends in academic progress, 2012. Washington D.C. Retrieved from [URL].
National Early Literacy Panel. (2009). Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel. Jessup, Maryland: National Institute for Literacy.
Neuman, S. B., Newman, E. H., & Dwyer, J. (2011). Educational effects of a vocabulary intervention on preschoolers' word knowledge and conceptual development: A cluster-randomized trial. Reading Research Quarterly, 46(3), 249–272.
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2005). A day in third grade: A large-scale study of classroom quality and teacher and student behavior. Elementary School Journal, 105(3), 305–323. 

NICHD ECCRN. (2005). Pathways to reading: The role of oral language in the transition to reading. Developmental Psychology, 41(2), 428–442. 

Oakhill, J. V., & Cain, K. (2012). The precursors of reading ability in young readers: Evidence from a four-year longitudinal study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(2), 91–121. 

Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development. (2012). Education at a glance 2012: OECD indicators. OECD Publishing. Retrieved from 

Ouellette, G. P. (2006). What's meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(3), 554–566. 

Roskos, K., & Burstein, K. (2011). Assessment of the design efficacy of a preschool vocabulary instruction technique. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 25, 268–287. 

Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., & Cooper, D. H. (2002). A longitudinal analysis of the connection between oral language and early reading. The Journal of Educational Research, 95(5), 259–272. 

Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development, 61, 1728–1734. 

Scarborough, H. S. (1991). Early syntactic development of dyslexic children. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 207–220. 

Scarborough, H. S. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities. In S. B. Neuman, & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp.97–110). New YorK, NY: Guilford Press.
Snow, C. E. (1991). The theoretical basis for relationships between language and literacy in development. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6(1), 5–10. 

Snow, C. E., & Beals, D. E. (2006). Mealtime talk that supports literacy development. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2006 (111), 52–66. 

Snow, C. E., & Dickinson, D. K. (1991). Skills that aren't basic in a new conception of literacy. In A. C. Purves, & E. Jennings (Eds.), Literate systems and individual lives: Perspectives on literacy and school. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
Snow, C. E., Tabors, P. O., Nicholson, P. A., & Kurland, B. F. (1995). SHELL: Oral language and early literacy skills in kindergarten and first-grade children. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 10(1), 37–48. 

Speece, D. L., Ritchey, K. D., Cooper, D. H., Roth, F. P., & Schatschneider, C. (2004). Growth in early reading skills from kindergarten to third grade. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29(3), 312–332. 

Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. G. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In R. O. Freedle (Ed.), New directions in discourse processing, Vol. 2 (pp.134 – 168). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934–947. 

Swanson, H. L., Rosston, K., Gerber, M., & Solari, E. (2008). Influence of oral language and phonological awareness on children's bilingual reading. Journal of School Psychology, 46(4), 413–429. 

Tilstra, J., McMaster, K., van den Broek, P., Kendeou, P., & Rapp, D. (2009). Simple but complex: Components of the simple view of reading across grade levels. Journal of Research in Reading, 32(4), 383–401. 

Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S. R., & Hecht, S. A. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second-to-fifth grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161–185. 

Trabasso, T. (2005). Goal plans of action and inferences during comprehension of narratives. Discourse Processes, 39(2–3), 129–164. 

Tunmer, W. E., & Chapman, J. W. (2012). The simple view of reading redux: Vocabulary knowledge and the independent components hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(5), 453–466. 

Van Den Broek, P., Virtue, S., Everson, M. G., Tzeng, Y., & Sung, Y. (2002). Comprehension and memory of science texts: Inferential processes and the construction of a mental representation. In J. Otero, J. A. L. Gascón, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), The psychology of science text comprehension (pp.131–154). Mahwah, NJ: Lawerence Erlbaum.
Van den Broek, P., Young, M., Tzeng, Y., & Linderholm, T. (1999). The landscape model of reading: Inferences and the online construction of a memory representation. In J. Otero, J. A. L. Gascón, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), The psychology of science text comprehension (pp.71–98). Mahwah, NJ: Lawerence Erlbaum.
Vellutino, F. R., Tunmer, W. E., Jaccard, J. J., & Chen, R. S. (2007). Components of reading ability: Multivariate evidence for a convergent skills model of reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(1), 3–32. 

Verhoeven, L., van Leeuwe, J., & Vermeer, A. (2011). Vocabulary growth and reading development across the elementary school years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(1), 8–25. 

Walker, D., Greenwood, C., Hart, B., & Carta, J. (1994). Prediction of school outcomes based on early language production and socioeconomic factors. Child Development, 65, 606–621. 

Wasik, B. A., Bond, M. A., & Hindman, A. (2006). The effects of a language and literacy intervention on Head Start children and teachers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 63–74. 

Weisberg, D. S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R., Nicolopoulou, A., & Dickinson, D. K. (2015). Shovels and swords: How realistic and fantastical themes affect children's word learning. Cognitive Development, 35, 1–14. 

Wendling, B. J., Schrank, F. A., & Schmitt, A. J. (2007). Educational interventions related to the Woodcock-Johnson III tests of achievement (Assessment Service Bulletin No. 8). Rolling Meadows, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Williams, K. T. (1997). Expressive Vocabulary Test (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III tests of achievement. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Evatt, R. (2002). Preschool Language Scale - III. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Cited by (3)
Cited by three other publications
Kambach, Anna Elizabeth & Heidi Anne Mesmer
2024.
Comprehension for Emergent Readers: Revisiting the Reading Rope.
The Reading Teacher 77:6
► pp. 888 ff.

Zaretsky, Elena & Susie Russak
2024.
Using oral narratives to examine acquisition of English verb morphology among multilingual Arabic and monolingual Hebrew speakers: finding similarities with monolingual English-speaking SLIs.
International Journal of Multilingualism 21:4
► pp. 2217 ff.

Russak, Susie & Elena Zaretsky
2021.
Cognitive and Linguistic Skills Associated With Cross-Linguistic Transfer in the Production of Oral Narratives in English as a Foreign Language by Arabic- and Hebrew-Speaking Children: Finding Common Denominators.
Frontiers in Psychology 12

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 2 november 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.