Article published In:
Studies in Language
Vol. 41:4 (2017) ► pp.872913
References (71)
References
Adamou, Evangelia. 2014. L’antipassif en ixcatèque. Bulletin de la Société de Linguistique de Paris 109(1). 373–396.Google Scholar
. 2016. A corpus-driven approach to language contact. Endangered languages in a comparative perspective. Berlin & Boston: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2017. Spatial language and cognition among the Ixcatec-Spanish bilinguals (Mexico). In Kate Bellamy, Mike Child, Antje Muntendam & M. Carmen Parafita Couto (eds.), Multidisciplinary approaches to bilingualism in the Hispanic and Lusophone world, 175–209. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adamou, Evangelia & Denis Costaouec. 2013. El complementante la en ixcateco: marcador de clausula relativa, completiva y adverbial. Amerindia 37(1). 193–210.Google Scholar
Aissen, Judith. 1996. Pied-piping, abstract agreement, and functional projections in Tzotzil. Natural language and linguistic theory 14(3). 447‒491. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alexopoulou, Theodora. 2006. Resumption in relative clauses. Natural language and linguistic theory 24(1). 57‒111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anand, Pranav, Sandra Chung & Matthew Wagers. 2011. Widening the net: Challenges for gathering linguistic data in the digital age. Submitted to the National Science Foundation SBE 2020 planning activity. [URL] (accessed 16 January 2017).
Andrews, Avery D. 2007. Relative clauses. In Timothy Shopen (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description, 206–236. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Asudeh, Ash. 2012. The logic of pronominal resumption. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Martin Maechler, Ben Bolker & Steve Walker. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67(1). 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borer, Hagit. 1984. Restrictive relatives in modern Hebrew. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 2(2). 219–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borja, Manuel F., Sandra Chung & Matthew Wagers. 2016. Constituent order and parser control processes in Chamorro. In Amber Camp, Yuko Otsuka, Claire Stabile & Nozomi Tanaka (eds.), Proceedings of the 21st Annual Meeting of the Austronesian Formal Linguistics Association, 15–32. Canberra: Asia-Pacific Linguistics.Google Scholar
Caplan, David, Nathaniel Alpert & Gloria Waters. 1999. PET studies of syntactic processing with auditory sentence presentation. NeuroImage 9(3). 343–351. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caplan, David, Nathaniel Alpert, Gloria Waters & Anthony Olivieri. 2000. Activation of Broca’s area by syntactic processing under conditions of concurrent articulation. Human Brain Mapping 9(2). 65–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caplan, David, Sujith Vijayan, Gina Kuperberg, Caroline West, Gloria Waters, Doug Greve & Anders M. Dale. 2001. Vascular responses to syntactic processing: Event related fMRI study of relative clauses. Human Brain Mapping 15(1). 26–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carreiras, Manuel, Jon Andoni Duñabeitia, Marta Vergara, Irene de la Cruz-Pavía & Itziar Laka. 2010. Subject relative clauses are not universally easier to process: Evidence from Basque. Cognition 115(1). 79–92. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clemens, Lauren Eby, Jessica Coon, Pedro Mateo Pedro, Adam Milton Morgan, Maria Polinsky, Gabrielle Tandet & Matthew Wagers. 2015. Ergativity and the complexity of extraction: A view from Mayan. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 33(2). 417–469. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costaouec, Denis & Michael Swanton. 2015. Classification nominale en ixcatèque. La linguistique 51(2). 201–239. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Desmet, Timothy & Edward Gibson. 2003. Disambiguation preferences and corpus frequencies in noun phrase conjunction. Journal of Memory and Language 49(3). 353–374. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dryer, Matthew. 2011. Order of subject and verb. In Matthew Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures online. Munich, Max Planck Digital Library. [URL] (accessed 6 July 2014).
Duffield, Cecily Jill & Laura A. Michaelis. 2011. Why subject relatives prevail: Constraints versus constructional licensing. Language and Cognition 3(2). 171–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernández de Miranda, María Teresa. 1951. Reconstrucción del protopopoloca. Revista Mexicana de Estudios Antropológicos 121. 61–93.Google Scholar
. 1953. Las formas posesivas del ixcateco. Memoria del Congreso Científico Mexicano 121. 159–170.Google Scholar
. 1956. Glotocronología de la familia popoloca. México, DF: Instituto Nacional de Antropología e Historia.Google Scholar
. 1959. Fonémica del Ixcateco. México, DF: Instituto nacional de antropología e historia (INAH).Google Scholar
. 1961. Diccionario ixcateco. México, DF: Instituto nacional de antropología e historia (INAH).Google Scholar
Frazier, Lyn. 1987. Syntactic processing: Evidence from Dutch. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 5(4). 519–559. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedmann, Naama & Rama Novogrodsky. 2004. The acquisition of relative clause comprehension in Hebrew: A study of SLI and normal development. Journal of Child Language 31(3). 661–681. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson, Edward. 1998. Linguistic complexity: locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition 68(1). 1–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, Peter C. & Randall Hendrick. 2005. Relativization, ergativity, and corpus frequency. Linguistic Inquiry 36(3). 456–463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grinevald, Colette. 2000. A morphosyntactic typology of classifiers. In Gunter Senft (ed.), Systems of nominal classification, 50‒92. Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gudschinsky, Sarah C. 1959. Proto-Popotecan. A comparative study of Popolocan and Mixtecan. Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University Publications.Google Scholar
Hamp, Eric P. 1958. Chocho-Popoloca innovations. International Journal of American Linguistics 26(1). 62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, John A. 1999. Processing complexity and filler-gap dependencies across grammars. Language 75(2). 244‒285. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hironymous, Michael. 2007. Santa Maria Ixcatlan, Oaxaca: From colonial Cacicazgo to modern Municipio. Austin, TX: University of Texas PhD dissertation.Google Scholar
Hsiao, Franny & Edward Gibson. 2003. Processing relative clauses in Chinese. Cognition 90(1). 3–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Just, Marcel A., Patricia A. Carpenter, Timothy A. Keller, William F. Eddy & Keith R. Thulborn. 1996. Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science 274(4). 114–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keenan, Edward L. & Bernard Comrie. 1977. Noun phrase accessibility and universal grammar. Linguistic Inquiry 8(1). 63–99.Google Scholar
King, Jonathan & Marcel A. Just. 1991. Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language 30(5). 580–602. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
King, Jonathan & Marta Kutas. 1995. Who did what and when? Using word- and clause-level ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 7(3). 376–395. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krauss, Michael. 2006. Classification and terminology for degrees of languages endangerment. In Matthias Brenzinger (ed.), Language diversity endangered, 1–8. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Kwon, Nayoung, Yoonhyoung Lee, Peter C. Gordon, Robert Kluender & Maria Polinsky. 2010. Cognitive and linguistic factors affecting subject/object asymmetry: An eye-tracking study of pre-nominal relative clauses in Korean. Language 86(3). 546–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kwon, Nayoung, Robert Kluender, Marta Kutas & Maria Polinsky. 2013. Subject/object processing asymmetries in Korean relative clauses: Evidence from ERP data. Language 89(3). 537–585. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, Christian. 1984. Der Relativsatz. Typologie seiner Strukturen, Theorie seiner Funktionen, Kompendium seiner Grammatik. Tübingen: G. Narr.Google Scholar
. 1986. On the typology of relative clauses. Linguistics 24(4). 663–680. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, Maryellen C. 2013. How language production shapes language form and comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology 41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, Maryellen C. & Morten Christiansen. 2002. Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1999). Psychological Review 109(1). 35–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian & Csaba Pleh. 1988. The processing of restrictive relative clauses in Hungarian. Cognition 29(2). 95–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Malchukov, Andrej, Martin Haspelmath & Bernard Comrie (eds). 2010. Ditransitive constructions: a typological overview. In Andrej Malchukov, Martin Haspelmath & Bernard Comrie (eds.), Studies in ditransitive constructions: A comparative handbook, 1–64. Berlin & New York: De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McCloskey, James. 1990. Resumptive pronouns, Aʹ-binding and levels of representation in Irish. In Randall Hendrick (ed.), The syntax of the modern Celtic languages, 199‒248. New York: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mecklinger, Axel, Herbert Schriefers, Karsten Steinhauer & Angela Friederici. 1995. Processing relative clauses varying on syntactic and semantic dimensions: An analysis with event-related potentials. Memory and Cognition 23(4). 477–494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miyamoto, Edson T. & Michiko Nakamura. 2003. Subject/object asymmetries in the processing of relative clauses in Japanese. In Gina Garding & Mimu Tsujimura (eds.), Proceedings of the 22nd West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, 342–355. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Nichols, Johanna. 1986. Head-marking and dependent-marking grammar. Language 62(1). 56–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, Maria. 2008. Relative clauses in heritage Russian: Fossilization or divergent grammar? In Andrei Antonenko, John F. Bailyn & Christina Y. Bethin (eds.), Formal Approaches to Slavic Linguistics (FASL) 16: The Stony Brook Meeting 2007, 333–357. University of Michigan: Michigan Slavic Publications.Google Scholar
Polinsky, Maria, Carlos Gomez-Gallo, Peter Graff & Ekaterina Kravtchenko. 2012. Subject preference and ergativity. Lingua 122(3). 267–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. 2013. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. [URL].
Reali, Florencia & Morten H. Christiansen. 2007. Processing of relative clauses is made easier by frequency of occurrence. Journal of Memory and Language 57(1). 1–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, Florian. 2007. Processing presupposed content. Journal of Semantics 24(4). 373–416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shlonsky, Ur. 1992. Resumptive pronouns as a last resort. Linguistic Inquiry 23(3). 443‒448.Google Scholar
Skopeteas, Stavros, Ines Fiedler, Samantha Hellmuth, Anne Schwarz, Ruben Stoel & Gisbert Fanselow. 2006. Questionnaire on Information Structure. Potsdam: Audiovisuelles Zentrum der Universität Potsdam und sd:k Satz Druck GmbH Potsdam.Google Scholar
Suñer, Margarita. 1998. Resumptive restrictive relatives: A cross-linguistic perspective. Language 74(2). 335–364. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Swadesh, Morris. 1960. The Oto-Manguean hypothesis and macro Mixtecan. International Journal of American Linguistics 26(2). 79–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Traxler, Matthew J., Robin K. Morris & Rachel E. Seely. 2002. Processing subject and object relative clauses: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language 47(1). 69–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ueno, Mieko & Susan M. Garnsey. 2008. An ERP study of the processing of subject and object relative clauses in Japanese. Language and Cognitive Processes 23(5). 646–688. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vasishth, Shravan, Zhong Chen, Qiang Li & Gueilan Guo. 2013. Processing Chinese relative clauses: Evidence for the subject-relative advantage. PLoS ONE 8(10). e77006. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Veerman-Leichsenring, Annette. 2000. Popolocan independent personal pronouns: Comparison and reconstruction. International Journal of American Linguistics 66(3). 318–359. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2001a. Ixcateco: La frase nominal. Anales de Antropología 351. 323–358.Google Scholar
. 2001b. Changes in Popolocan word order and clause structure. In Jan T. Faarlund (ed.), Grammatical relations in change, 303–322. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2001c. Coreference in the Popolocan languages. In Laurel J. Brinton (ed.), Historical linguistics 1999: Selected papers from the 14th International Conference on Historical Linguistics, Vancouver, 9–13 August 1999, 337–350. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Warren, Tessa & Edward Gibson. 2002. The influence of referential processing on sentence complexity. Cognition 85(1). 79–112. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wells, Justine, Morten Christiansen, David Race, Daniel Acheson & Maryellen C. MacDonald. 2009. Experience and sentence comprehension: Statistical learning and relative clause comprehension. Cognitive Psychology 58(2). 250–271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (2)

Cited by two other publications

Calderón, Eréndira, Stefano De Pascale & Evangelia Adamou
2019. How to speak “geocentric” in an “egocentric” language: A multimodal study among Ngigua-Spanish bilinguals and Spanish monolinguals in a rural community of Mexico. Language Sciences 74  pp. 24 ff. DOI logo
Adamou, Evangelia, Matthew Gordon & Stefan Th. Gries
2018. Chapter 3. Prosodic and morphological focus marking in Ixcatec (Otomanguean). In Information Structure in Lesser-described Languages [Studies in Language Companion Series, 199],  pp. 51 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 18 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.