Article published In:
Scientific Study of Literature
Vol. 12:1/2 (2022) ► pp.133163
References (38)
References
Bal, P. M., & Veltkamp, M. (2013). How does fiction reading influence empathy? An experimental investigation on the role of emotional transportation. PLOS ONE. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barsky, R. F. (2021). Clamouring for legal protection: What the great books teach us about people fleeing from persecution. UK Hart Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Currie, G. P. (2013). Does fiction civilize us? New York Times, June 1 [URL]
Daniller, A. (2019). American’s immigration policy priorities: Divisions between – and within – the two parties. Pew Research Center, Nov. 12, [URL]
Davis, M. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10 1, 85.Google Scholar
(1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44 1, 113–126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Djikic, M., Oatley, K., & Moldoveanu, M. C. (2013). Reading other minds: Effects of literature on empathy. Scientific Study of Literature, 3 (1), 28–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, D. R. (2012). Transportation into a story increases empathy, prosocial behavior, and perceptual bias toward fearful expressions. Personality and Individual Differences, 52 (2), 150–155. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, D. R., Cushman, G. K., Borden, L. A., & McCune, M. S. (2013). Potentiating empathic growth: Generating imagery while reading fiction increases empathy and prosocial behavior. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7 (3), 306. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, D. R., Huffman, B. L., & Jasper, D. M. (2014). Changing race boundary perception by reading narrative fiction. Basic and Applied Psychology, 36 1, 83–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, G., & Libby, L. (2012). Changing beliefs and behavior through experience-taking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103 (1), 1–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kidd, D. C., & Castano, E. (2013). Reading literary fiction improves theory of mind. Science, 342 (6156), 377–380. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kim, N., Kim, H., Wojcieszak, M. K., Igartua, J-J., & Lim, M. (2020). The presence of the protagonist: Explaining narrative perspective effects through social presence. Media Psychology, 23 : 6,891–914. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Klein, E. (2021). Why we’re polarized. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Koopman, E. M. (2015). Empathic reactions after reading: The role of genre, personal factors and affective responses. Poetics, 50 1, 62–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langkau, L. (2020). The empathic skill fiction can’t teach us. Philosophical Psychology, 33 (3), 313–331, DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lenhart, J., Dangel, J., & Richter, T. (2020). The relationship between lifetime book reading and empathy in adolescents: examining transportability as a moderator. Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts, Online First Publication, August 13, 2020. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mar, R. A., Oatley, K., Hirsh, J., Dela Paz, J., & Peterson, J. B. (2006). Bookworms versus nerds: Exposure to fiction versus non-fiction, divergent associations with social ability, and the simulation of fictional social worlds. Journal of Research in Personality, 40 1, 694–712. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mar, R. A. (2011). The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. The Annual Review of Psychology, 62 1:103–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018). Evaluating whether stories can promote social cognition: Introducing the Social Processes and Content Entrained by Narrative (SPaCEN) framework. Discourse Processes, 55:5–6, 454–479, DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martinez, J. E., Feldman, L. A., Feldman, M. J., & Cikara, M. (2021). Narratives shape cognitive representations of immigrants and immigration-policy preferences. Psychological Science, 32 (2), 135–152. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McConahay, J. B. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the modern racism scale. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 91–125). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mumper, M. L., & Gerrig, R. J. (2017). Leisure reading and social cognition: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11 (1), 109–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nussbaum, M. (1997). Cultivating humanity: A classical defense of reform in liberal education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Oatley, K. (2016). Fiction: Simulation of social worlds. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20 (8), 618–628. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Panero, M. E., Weisberg, D. S., Black, J., Goldstein, T. R., Barnes, J. L., Brownell, H., & Winner, E. (2016). Does reading a single passage of literary fiction really improve theory of mind? An attempt at replication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111 (5): 46–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, T. F., Tropp, L. R., Wagner, U., & Christ, O. (2011). Recent advances in intergroup contact theory. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 35 1, 31, 271–280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pino, M. C., & Mazza, M. (2016). The use of “literary fiction” to promote mentalizing ability. PLOS ONE, 11 (8), 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rorty, R. (1989). Contingency, irony, and solidarity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Samur, D., Tops, M. & Koole, S. L. (2018). Does a single session of reading literary fiction prime enhance mentalising performance? Four replication experiments of Kidd and Castano (2013) . Cognition and Emotion, 32 (1), 130–144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stiles, S. (2014). Redemption, Rorty, and Nathanael West’s Miss Lonelyhearts . Interdisciplinary Literary Studies, 16 (2), 239–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Suárez-Orozco, C., & Suárez-Orozco, M. M. (2009). Children of immigration. Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vargas, J. A. (2018). Dear America: Notes of an undocumented citizen. New York: Dey Street Books (imprint of HarperCollins).Google Scholar
Vezzali, L., Stathi, S., Giovannini, E., Capozza, D., & Trifiletti, E. (2015). The greatest magic of Harry Potter: Reducing prejudice. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 45 (2), 105–121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, L., Currie, G., Friend, S., & Ferguson, H. (2021). The effects of reading narrative fiction on social and moral cognition. Scientific Study of Literature, 11 (2), 223–265. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Winner, E. (2019). How art works: A psychological exploration. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zunshine, L. (2012). Why we read fiction: Theory of mind and the novel. Columbus, OH: Ohio State University Press.Google Scholar