Part of
Theories of Reading Development
Edited by Kate Cain, Donald L. Compton and Rauno K. Parrila
[Studies in Written Language and Literacy 15] 2017
► pp. 239256
References
Adams, M. J.
(1990) Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT.Google Scholar
Anderson, R., & Freebody, P.
(1981) Vocabulary knowledge. In J. T. Guthrie (Ed.), Comprehension and teaching: Research reviews (pp. 77–117). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
Anglin, J. M.
(1993) Knowing versus learning words. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, 176–186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beck, I., & McKeown, M.
(1991) Conditions of vocabulary acquisition. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 789–814). New York: Longman.Google Scholar
Beck, I. L., Perfetti, C. A., & McKeown, M. G.
(1982) Effects of long-term vocabulary instruction on lexical access and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 506–521. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Braze, D., Tabor, W., Shankweiler, D. P., & Mencl, W. E.
(2007) Speaking up for vocabulary in primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 44–62.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. L.
(1988) Morphology as lexical organization. In M. Hammond & M. Noonan (Eds.), Theoretical morphology (pp. 119–141). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cain, K.
(2007) Deriving word meanings from context: Does explanation facilitate contextual analysis? Journal of Research in Reading, 30, 347–359. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J.
(2011) Matthew effects in young readers: Reading comprehension and reading experience aid vocabulary development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44, 431–443. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cain, K. & Oakhill, J.
(2014) Reading comprehension and vocabulary: Is vocabulary more important for some aspects of comprehension? L’Année Psychologique, 114, 647–662. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P.
(2004) Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 31–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F.
(1988) Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth, and eighth graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 247–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995) Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In L. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological apsects of language processing (pp. 189–209). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(2000) Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Journal of Reading and Writing, 12, 169–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003) Morphology matters in learning to read: A commentary. Reading Psychology, 24, 373–404. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Fostering morphological processing, vocabulary development, and reading comprehension. In R. K. Wagner, A. E. Muse, & K. R. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 78–103). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. & Nomanbhoy, D. M.
(1993) Phonological and morphological awareness in first graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 177–195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W., & Tannen, D.
(1987) The relation between written and spoken language. Annual Review of Anthropology, 16, 383–407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coyne, M. D., McCoach, D. B., Loftus, S., Zipoli, R., Jr., & Kapp, S.
(2009) Direct vocabulary instruction in kindergarten: Teaching for breadth versus depth. The Elementary School Journal, 110, 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M.
(1988) Word knowledge and reading skills. In M. Daneman, G. MacKinnon, & T. G. Waller (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (Vol. 6, pp. 145–175). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
de Jong, P., & van der Leij, A.
(2002) Effects of phonological abilities and linguistic comprehension on the development of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 51–77. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquarella, A.
(2013) Reciprocal relationship: Children’s morphological awareness and their reading accuracy across grades 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1113–1126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Kieffer, M. J., & Laroche, A.
(2014) The relation between morphological awareness and reading comprehension: Evidence from mediation and longitudinal models. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 432–451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H. & Kirby, J.
(2004) Morphological awareness: Just more phonological? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development, Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 223–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dixon, P., LeFevre, J. A., & Twilley, L. C.
(1988) Word knowledge and working memory as predictors of reading skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 465–472. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elleman, A. M., Lindo, E. J., Morphy, P., & Compton, D. L.
(2009) The impact of vocabulary instruction on passage level comprehension of school age children: A meta-analysis. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 2, 1–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Farkas, G. & Beron, K.
(2004) The detailed age trajectory of oral vocabulary knowledge: Differences by class and race. Social Science Research, 33, 464–97. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fellbaum, C.
(1998, ed.) WordNet: An Electronic Lexical Database. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ferrer, E. & McArdle, J.
(2010) Longitudinal modeling of developmental changes in psychological research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 149–154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferrer, E., McArdle, J. J., Shaywitz, B. A., Holahan, J. M., Marchione, K., & Shaywitz, S. E.
(2007) Longitudinal models of developmental dynamics between reading and cognition from childhood to adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1460–1473. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferrer, E., Shaywitz, B. A., Holahan, J. M., Marchione, K., & Shaywitz, S. E.
(2010) Uncoupling of reading and IQ over time: Empirical evidence for a definition of dyslexia. Psychological Science, 21, 93–101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ford, M. A., Davis, M. H., & Marslen-Wilson, W.
(2010) Derivational morphology and base morpheme frequency. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 117–130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E.
(1986) Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B.
(1990) The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2, 127–160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kamil, M. L., & Hiebert, E. H.
(2005) Teaching and learning vocabulary: Perspectives and persistent issues. In E. Hiebert & M. Kamil (Eds.), Teaching and learning vocabulary: Bringing research to practice (pp. 1–23). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kieffer, M. J., & Lesaux, N. K.
(2008) The role of derivational morphological awareness in the reading comprehension of Spanish-speaking English language learners. Reading and Writing, 21, 783–804. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kieffer, M. J. & Lesaux, N. K.
(2012a) Development of morphological awareness and vocabulary knowledge in Spanish-speaking language minority learners: A parallel process latent growth curve model. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33, 23–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kieffer, M. J., & Lesaux, N. K.
(2012b) Knowledge of words, knowledge about words: Dimensions of vocabulary in first and second language learners in sixth grade. Reading and Writing, 25, 347–373. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012c) Direct and indirect roles of morphological awareness in the English reading comprehension of native English, Spanish, Filipino, and Vietnamese speakers. Language Learning, 62, 1170–1204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. H., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Woolley, L., & Parrila, R.
(2012) Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing, 25, 389–410. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kruk, R. S., & Bergman, K.
(2013) The reciprocal relations between morphological processes and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 10–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lehr, F., Osborn, J., & Hiebert, E. H.
(2004) Research-based practices in early reading series: Focus on vocabulary. Pacific Resources for Education and Learning PREL.Google Scholar
Long, M.
(2001) Psychology of education. United Kingdom: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lonigan, C. J.
(2007) Vocabulary development and the development of phonological awareness skills in preschool children. In R. Wagner, A. Muse & K. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 15–31). New York: Guilford PressGoogle Scholar
Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Anthony, J. L.
(2000) Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596–613. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McArdle, J. J.
(2009) Latent variable modeling of longitudinal data. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 577–605. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Wagner, R. K., Muse, A., Chow, B. W.-Y., & Shu, H.
(2005) The role of morphological awareness in children’s vocabulary acquisition in English. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 415–435. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McKeown, M. G., Beck, I. L., Omanson, R. C., & Perfetti, C. A.
(1983) The effects of long-term vocabulary instruction on reading comprehension: A replication. Journal of Literacy Research, 15, 3–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McKeown, M. G., Beck, I. L., Omanson, R. C., & Pople, M. T.
(1985) Some effects of the nature and frequency of vocabulary instruction on the knowledge and use of words. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 522–535. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mehta, P. D., Foorman, B. R., Branum-Martin, L., & Taylor, W. P.
(2005) Literacy as a unidimensional multilevel construct: Validation, sources of influence, and implications in a longitudinal study in grades 1 to 4. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 85–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A.
(1995) WordNet: A Lexical Database for English. Communications of the ACM Vol. 38, No. 11: 39–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. J., & Stevenson, J.
(2004) Phonemes, rimes, and language skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 663–681. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W.
(2007) Metalinguistic awareness and the vocabulary-comprehension connection. In R. K. Wager, A. E. Muse, & K. R. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 52–77). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Nagy, W. E., & Anderson, R. C.
(1984) How many words are there in printed school English? Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304–330. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W. E., Berninger, V., & Abbott, R.
(2006) Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98,134–147. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W. E., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Vaughn, K., & Vermeulen, K.
(2003) Relationship of morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk second grade readers and at-risk fourth grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 730–742. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W. E. & Herman, P. A.
(1987) Breadth and depth of vocabulary knowledge: Implications for acquisition and instruction. In M. McKeown and M. Curtis (Eds.), The Nature of Vocabulary Acquisition, (pp. 19–35). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. A., & Anderson, R.
(1985) Learning words from context. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 304–330. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W. E., & Scott, J. A.
(2000) Vocabulary Processes. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research, Vol III (pp. 269–284). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Nation, K., Snowling, M. J., & Clarke, P.
(2007) Dissecting the relationship between language skills and learning to read: Semantic and phonological contributions to new vocabulary learning in children with poor reading comprehension. Advances in Speech Language Pathology, 9, 131–139. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
(2000) Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. (NIH Publication No. 00–4769). Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.Google Scholar
Olofsson, A. & Niedersoe, J.
(1999) Early language development and kindergarten phonological awareness as predictors of reading problems. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 464–472. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ouellette, G.
(2006) What’s meaning got to do with it: the role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 554–566. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ouellette, G., & Beers, A.
(2010) A not-so-simple view of reading: How oral vocabulary and visual-word recognition complicate the story. Reading and Writing, 23, 189–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L.
(2001) The lexical bases of comprehension skill. In D. Gorfien (Ed.), On the consequences of meaning selection (pp. 67–86). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C., & Hart, L.
(2002) The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189–213). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Phythian-Sence, C., & Wagner, R. K.
(2007) Vocabulary acquisition: A primer. In R. Wagner, A. Muse, & K. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 1–14). New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Quinn, J. M., Wagner, R. K., Petscher, Y., & Lopez, D.
(2015) Developmental relations between vocabulary knowledge and reading comprehension: A latent change score modeling study. Child Development, 86, 159–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, M. R., & Turek, J. J.
(2012) A dynamic developmental link between verbal comprehension-knowledge (Gc) and reading comprehension: Verbal comprehension knowledge drives positive change in reading comprehension. Journal of School Psychology, 50, 841–863. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sénéchal, M., Ouellette, G., & Rodney, D.
(2006) The misunderstood giant: On the predictive role of early vocabulary to future reading. In S. B. Neuman & D. Dickinson (Eds.) Handbook of early literacy research. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H.
(2006) Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 122–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spencer, M., Muse, A., Wagner, R. K., Foorman, B., Petscher, Y., Schatschneider, C., Tighe, E. L, & Bishop, D.
(2015) Examining the underlying dimensions of morphological awareness and vocabulary knowledge. Reading and Writing, 28, 959–988. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stahl, S. A., & Fairbanks, M. M.
(1986) The effects of vocabulary instruction: A model-based meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 56, 72–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E.
(1992) Speculations on the causes and consequence of individual differences in early reading acquisition. In P. Gough, L. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 307–342). Mahwah, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Tannenbaum, K. R., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K.
(2006) Relationships between word knowledge and reading comprehension in third-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 381–398. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tong, X., Deacon, S. H., Kirby, J. R., Cain, K., & Parrila, R.
(2011) Morphological awareness: A key to understanding poor reading comprehension in English. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 523–534. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tunmer, W., Herriman, M., & Nesdale, A.
(1988) Metalinguistic abilities and beginning reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 134–158. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tyler A. & Nagy, W. E.
(1989) The acquisition of English derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 649–667. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tyler, A. & W. Nagy
(1990) Use of English derivational morphology during reading. Cognition, 36, 17–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verhoeven, L., & van Leeuwe, J.
(2008) Prediction of the development of reading comprehension: A longitudinal study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 407–423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verhoeven, L., van Leeuwe, J., & Vermeer, A.
(2011) Vocabulary growth and reading development across the elementary school years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 8–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Meros, D.
(2010) Vocabulary and reading comprehension: direct, indirect, and reciprocal influences. Focus on Exceptional Children, 43, 1–10.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Muse, A. E., & Tannenbaum, K. R.
(Eds.) (2007) Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K.
(1987) The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Laughon, P., Simmons, K., & Rashotte, C. A.
(1993) Development of young readers’ phonological processing abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 1–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A.
(1994) The development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., & Pearson, N. A.
(2013) Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing – Second Edition. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.Google Scholar
Windsor, J.
(1994) Children’s comprehension and production of derivational suffixes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 37, 408–417. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wise, J. C., Sevcil, R. A., Morris, R. D., Lovett, M. W., & Wolf, M.
(2007) The relationship among receptive and expressive vocabulary, listening comprehension, pre-reading skills, word identification skills, and reading comprehension by children with reading disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 1093–1109. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 8 other publications

Castles, Anne, Kathleen Rastle & Kate Nation
2018. Ending the Reading Wars: Reading Acquisition From Novice to Expert. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 19:1  pp. 5 ff. DOI logo
Chan, Jessica S., Lesly Wade-Woolley, Lindsay Heggie & John R. Kirby
2020. Understanding prosody and morphology in school-age children’s reading. Reading and Writing 33:5  pp. 1295 ff. DOI logo
Gissel, Stig Toke & Simon Calmar Andersen
2021. A cluster‐randomized trial measuring the effects of a digital learning tool supporting decoding and reading for meaning in grade 2. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning 37:2  pp. 287 ff. DOI logo
Kotzer, Maddie, John R. Kirby & Lindsay Heggie
2021. Morphological Awareness Predicts Reading Comprehension in Adults. Reading Psychology 42:3  pp. 302 ff. DOI logo
Lee, Sung Hee
2023. Effects of Moderately Rich Vocabulary Instruction on Literacy Performances of Students With Reading Disabilities. Communication Disorders Quarterly DOI logo
Mason, Rihana S. & Lori A. Bass
2020. Just Ask Me Again: An Analysis of Receptive Vocabulary Performance of Children from Low-Income Environments. Early Education and Development 31:6  pp. 910 ff. DOI logo
Nevo, Einat
2024. The Effect of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Low SES Kindergarteners’ Language Abilities. Early Childhood Education Journal 52:3  pp. 459 ff. DOI logo
Rodríguez‐Ortiz, Isabel R., Francisco J. Moreno‐Pérez, Ian C. Simpson, Marta Valdés‐Coronel & David Saldaña
2021. The influence of syntactic knowledge on reading comprehension varies as a function of oral vocabulary in Spanish‐speaking children. Journal of Research in Reading 44:3  pp. 695 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.