Part of
Pragmatic Development in First Language Acquisition
Edited by Danielle Matthews
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 10] 2014
► pp. 161182
References (89)
Allen, S. (2000). A discourse-pragmatic explanation for argument representation in child Inuktitut. Linguistics , 38(3), 483–521. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Allen, S., Skarabela, B., & Hughes, M. (2008). Using corpora to examine discourse effects in syntax. In H. Behrens (Ed.), Corpora in Language Acquisition Research: Finding Structure in Data (pp. 99–137). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Anselmi, D., Tomasello, M. & Acunzo, M. (1986). Young children’s responses to neutral and specific contingent queries. Journal of Child Language , 13, 135–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ariel, M. (1990). Accessing Noun Phrase Antecedents . London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Arnold, J.E., Bennetto, L., & Diehl, J. (2009). Reference production in young speakers with and without autism: Effects of discourse status and processing constraints. Cognition , 110(2), 131–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arnold, J., Brown-Schmidt, S. & Trueswell, J. (2007). Children’s use of gender and order-of-mention during pronoun comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes , 22(4), 527–565. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baker, N.D., & Greenfield, P.M. (1988). The development of new and old information in young children’s early language. Language Sciences , 10(1), 3–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baltaxe, C. (1977). Pragmatic deficits in the language of autistic adolescents. Journal of Pediatric Psychology , 2, 176–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bahtiyar, S., & Kuntay, A.C. (2009). Integration of communicative partner’s visual perspective in patterns of referential requests. Journal of Child Language , 36, 529–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Begeer, S., Malle, B.F., Nieuwland, M.S., & Keysar, B. (2010). Using Theory of Mind to represent and take part in social interactions: Comparing individuals with high-functioning autism and typically developing controls. European Journal of Developmental Psychology , 7(1), 104–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brener, R. (1983). Learning the deictic meaning of third person pronouns. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research , 12, 235–262.Google Scholar
Brown, R., & Fraser, C. (1963). The acquisition of syntax. In Cofer, C.N., Musgrave, B. (Eds). Verbal Behaviour and Learning: Problems and Processes . New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Charman, T., & Shmueli-Goetz, Y. (1998). The relationship between theory of mind, language, and narrative discourse: An experimental study. Current Psychology of Cognition , 17, 245–271.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. (1976). Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, & point of view. In C.N. Li (Ed.), Subject and Topic (pp. 25–56). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
. (1994). Discourse, Consciousness, and Time: The Flow and Displacement of Conscious Experience in Speaking and Writing . Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Chien, Y.C., & Wexler, K. (1990). Children’s knowledge of locality conditions in binding as evidence for the modularity of syntax and pragmatics. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics , 1, 225–295. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clancy, P.M. (1993). Preferred argument structure in Korean acquisition. In E.V. Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Child Language Research Forum (pp. 307–314). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
. (2003). The lexicon in interaction: Developmental origins of preferred argument structure in Korean. In J. Du Bois, E. Kumpf, & W.J. Ashby (Eds.), Preferred Argument Structure: Grammar as Architecture for Function (pp. 81–108). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., & Schaefer, E.F. (1987). Collaborating on contributions to conversation. Language and Cognitive Processes , 2, 19–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. (1986). Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition , 22, 1–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., Schreuder, R., & Buttrick, S. (1983). Common ground and the understanding of demonstrative reference. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior , 22, 245–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., & Bangerter, A. (2004). Changing ideas about reference. In I.A. Noveck & D. ­Sperber (Eds.), Experimental Pragmatics (pp. 25–49). Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Dahlgren, S., & Sandberg, D.A. (2008). Referential communication in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice , 12(4), 335–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davies, C., & Katsos, N. (2010). Over-informative children: Production/comprehension asymmetry or tolerance to pragmatic violations? Lingua , 120(8), 1956–1972. Special Issue on Asymmetries in Child Language . DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Cat, C. (2011). Information tracking and encoding in early L1: Linguistic competence vs. cognitive limitations. Journal of Child Language , 38(4), 828–860. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, W., & Pechmann, T. (1982). Social-interaction and the development of definite descriptions. Cognition , 11(2), 159–184. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Villiers, J., Stainton, R.J. & Szatmari, P. (2007). Pragmatic abilities in autism spectrum disorder: A case study in philosophy and the empirical. Midwest Studies in Philosophy , 31(1), 292–316. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, K.E., & Demuth, K. (2011). Individual differences in pronoun reversal: Evidence from two longitudinal case studies. Journal of Child Language, 39(1), 1–30.Google Scholar
Ford, W., & Olson, D. (1975). The elaboration of the noun phrase in children’s description of objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 19(3), 371–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Girbau, D. (2001). Children’s referential communication failure: The ambiguity and abbreviation of messages. Journal of Language and Social Psychology , 20(1–2), 81–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glucksberg, S. & Krauss, R. (1967). What do people say after they have learned how to talk? Studies of development and referential communication. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly , 13, 309–316.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S., Krauss, R.M., & Higgins, E.T. (1975). The development of referential communication skills. In F.D. Horowitz (Ed.), Review of Child Development Vol. 4 . Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Graf, E., Theakston, A., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M. (in press). Subject and object omission in children’s early transitive constructions: A discourse-pragmatic approach. Applied Psycholinguistics .
Grice, H.P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and Semantics, 3: Speech Acts (pp. 41–58). New York, NY: Academic Press. Reprinted in Grice, H.P. (1989). Studies in the Way of Words (pp. 22–40). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Guerriero, S., Oshima-Takane, Y., & Kuriyama, Y. (2006). The development of referential choice in English and Japanese: A discourse-pragmatic perspective. Journal of Child Language , 33, 823–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gundel, J.K., Hedberg, N., & Zacharski, R. (1993). Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language , 69(2), 274–307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hughes, M., & Allen, S. (2013). The effect of individual discourse pragmatic features on referential choice in child English. Journal of Pragmatics . [URL]
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child , 2, 217–250.Google Scholar
. (1946). Irrelevant and metaphorical language. American Journal of Psychiatry , 103, 242–246.Google Scholar
Keysar, B., Barr, D.J., Balin, J.A., & Brauner, J.S. (2000). Taking perspective in conversation: The role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychological Science , 11(1), 32–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, A., Hobson, R.P., & Chiat, S. (1994). I, you, me and autism: An experimental study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders , 24, 155–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liebal, K., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Infants’ use of shared experience in declarative pointing. Infancy , 15, 545–556. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liebal, K., Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Infants use shared experience to interpret pointing gestures. Developmental Science , 12(2), 264–271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U. (2008). Before L1: A differentiated perspective on infant gestures. Gesture , 8(2), 180–196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loveland, K. (1984). Learning about points of view: Spatial perspective and the acquisition of “I/you”. Journal of Child Language , 11, 535–556. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loveland, K., Tunali, B., Kelley, M.L., & McEvoy, R.E. (1989). Referential communication and response adequacy in autism and Down’s syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics , 10, 301–313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Butcher, J., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Two- and four-year-olds learn to adapt referring expressions to context: Effects of distracters and feedback on referential communication. TopiCS , 4(2), 184–210.Google Scholar
Matthews, D., Theakston, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Pronoun co-referencing errors: Challenges for generativist and usage-based accounts. Cognitive Linguistics , 20(3), 599–626. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M. (2006). The effect of perceptual availability and prior discourse on young children’s use of referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics , 27, 403–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2007). How toddlers and preschoolers learn to uniquely identify referents for others: A training study. Child Development , 78(6), 1744–1759. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maratsos, M. (1974). Preschool children’s use of definite and indefinite articles. Child Development , 45, 446–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Menig-Peterson, C.L. (1975). The modification of communicative behaviour in preschool-aged children as a function of listener’s perspective. Child Development , 46, 1015–1018. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Level 1 perspective-taking at 24 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology , 24, 603–613. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2007). How 14- and 18-month-olds know what others have experienced. Developmental Psychology , 43(2), 309–317. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morisseau, T., Davies, C., & Matthews, D. (2013). How do 3- and 5-year-olds respond to over- and under-informative utterances? Journal of Pragmatics . [URL]
Nadig, A. & Sedivy, J. (2002). Evidence of perspective-taking constraints in children’s on-line reference resolution. Psychological Science , 13(4), 329–336. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nadig A., Vivanti, G., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Adaptation of object descriptions to a partner under increasing communicative demands: A comparison of children with and without autism. Autism Research, 2(6): 334–347.Google Scholar
Narasimhan, B., Budwig, N., & Murty, L. (2005). Argument realization in Hindi caregiver-child discourse. Journal of Pragmatics , 37, 461–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, B., Adamson, L., & Bakeman, R. (2008). Toddlers’ joint engagement experience facilitates preschoolers’ acquisition of Theory of Mind. Developmental Science , 11(6), 847–852. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nilsen, E.S., & Mangal, L. (in press). Which is important for preschoolers’ production and repair of statements: What the listener knows or what the listener says? Journal of Child Language .
Nilsen, E., & Fecica, A.M. (2011). A model of communicative perspective-taking for typical and atypical populations of children. Developmental Review , 31, 55–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nilsen, E., & Graham, S. (2009). The relations between children’s communicative perspective-taking and executive functioning. Cognitive Psychology , 58, 220–249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nilsen, E., Graham, S., Smith, S., & Chambers, C. (2008). Preschoolers’ sensitivity to referential ambiguity: Evidence for a dissociation between implicit understanding and explicit behavior. Developmental Science , 11(4), 556–562. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norbury, C.F. & Bishop, D.V.M. (2003). Narrative skills of children with communication impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders , 38, 287–313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Neill, D.K. & Topolovec, J. (2001). Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to the referential (in)efficacy of their own pointing gestures. Journal of Child Language , 28, 1–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Neill, D.K. (1996). Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to a parent’s knowledge state when making requests. Child Development , 67, 695–677. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. & Navarro, S. (2003). Subject realization and crosslinguistic interference in the bilingual acquisition of Spanish and English. Journal of Child Language , 30, 1–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1994). A social interactionist account of developing decontextualized narrative skill. Developmental Psychology , 30(6), 937–948. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pyykkönen, P., Matthews, D., & Järvikivi, J. (2010). Verb semantics affects children’s pronoun comprehension: Evidence form eye-movements. Language and Cognitive Processes , 25(1), 115–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberts, R., & Patterson, C. (1983). Perspective taking and referential communication: The question of correspondence reconsidered. Child Development , 54, 1005–1014. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Robinson, E., & Robinson, W. (1982). The advancement of children’s verbal referential communication skills: The role of metacognitive guidance. International Journal of Behavioral Development , 5, 329–355. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salomo, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Children’s ability to answer different types of questions. Journal of Child Language , 40(2), 469–491. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saylor, M.M., Baird, J.A., & Gallerani, C. (2006). Telling others what’s new: Preschoolers’ adherence to the given-new contract. Journal of Cognition and Development , 7(3), 341–379. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L., Sorace, A., & Paoli, S. (2004). Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax-pragmatics interface: Subjects and objects in English-Italian bilingual and monolingual acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition , 7(3), 183–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L. (2005). The role of discourse pragmatics in the acquisition of subjects in Italian. Applied Psycholinguistics , 26(3), 437–462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2007). Null and overt subjects at the syntax-discourse interface: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual acquisition. In J. van Kampen, S. Bauw, & M. Pinto (Eds.), Selected Papers from The Romance Turn II. LOT Occasional Series (pp. 181–200). Utrecht: LOT.Google Scholar
Song, H., & Fisher, C. (2007). Discourse prominence effects on 2.5-year-old children’s interpretation of pronouns. Lingua , 117, 1959–1987. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2005). Who’s “she”? Discourse structure influences preschoolers’ pronoun interpretation. Journal of Memory and Language , 52, 29–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sonnenschein, S., & Whitehurst, G.J. (1984). Developing referential communication – A hierarchy of skills. Child Development , 55(5), 1936–1945. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sonnenschein, S. (1988). The development of referential communication – Speaking to different listeners. Child Development , 59(3), 694–702. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Skarabela, B. (2007). Signs of early social cognition in children’s syntax: The case of joint attention in argument realization in child Inuktitut. Lingua , 117(11), 1837–1857. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1993). ‘What language reveals about the understanding of minds in children with autism’. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D.J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism (pp. 138–157). Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
. (1994). ‘Dissociations in form and function in the acquisition of language by autistic children’. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Constraints on Language Acquisition: Studies of Atypical Children (pp. 175–194). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
. (1995). ‘Once upon a ribbit’: Stories narrated by autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology , 13, 45–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2001). A re-examination of the theory of mind hypothesis of autism. In J. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. Zelazo (Eds.), The Development of Autism: Perspectives from Theory and Research (pp. 173–193). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Volden, J., Mulcahy, R.F., & Holdgrafer, G. (1997). Pragmatic language disorder and perspective taking in autistic speakers. Applied Psycholinguistics , 18(02): 181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G.J. (1976). Development of communication – changes with age and modeling. Child Development , 47(2), 473–482. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G.J., & Sonnenschein, S. (1981). The development of informative messages in referential communication: Knowing when vs. knowing how. In W.P. Dickson (Ed.), Children’s Oral Communication Skills . New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Wykes, T. (1981). Inference and children’s comprehension of pronouns. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 32, 264–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (6)

Cited by six other publications

Sah, Wen hui
2024. The use of referring expressions in Mandarin-speaking children with and without developmental language disorder. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 38:1  pp. 40 ff. DOI logo
Fernández-Flecha, María, Andrea Junyent & María Blume
2023. Pragmatic development in Peruvian children. Pragmatics & Cognition 30:2  pp. 377 ff. DOI logo
Thorson, Jill C., Lauren R. Franklin & James L. Morgan
2023. Role of Pitch in Toddler Looking toNewandGivenReferents in American English. Language Learning and Development 19:4  pp. 458 ff. DOI logo
Vasil, Jared
2023. A New Look at Young Children’s Referential Informativeness. Perspectives on Psychological Science 18:3  pp. 624 ff. DOI logo
Salazar-Orvig, Anne, Geneviève de Weck, Rouba Hassan & Annie Rialland
Malkin, Louise, Kirsten Abbot-Smith & David Williams
2018. Is verbal reference impaired in autism spectrum disorder? A systematic review. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments 3 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 7 november 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.