Part of
Language in Interaction: Studies in honor of Eve V. Clark
Edited by Inbal Arnon, Marisa Casillas, Chigusa Kurumada and Bruno Estigarribia
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 12] 2014
► pp. 83100
References (72)
References
Akhtar, N., Jipson, J., & Callanan, M. (2001). Learning words through overhearing. Child Development , 72, 416–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aksu-Koç, A. (1998). The role of input vs. universal predispositions in the emergence of tense-aspect morphology: Evidence from Turkish. First Language , 18, 255–280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anselmi, D., Tomasello, M., & Acunzo, M. (1986). Young children’s responses to neutral and specific contingent queries. Journal of Child Language , 13, 135–144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S., & Berman, R.A. (2003). The emergence of grammar: Early verbs and beyond. Journal of Child Language , 30, 845–877. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baker, N.D., & Nelson, K.E. (1984). Recasting and related conversational techniques for triggering syntactic advances in young children. First Language , 5, 3–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barnes, S., Gutfreund, M., Satterly, D., & Wells, G. (1983). Characteristics of adult speech which predict children’s language development. Journal of Child Language , 10, 65–84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bassano, D. (2000). Early development of nouns and verbs in French: Exploring the interface between lexicon and grammar. Journal of Child Language , 27, 521–559 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bertin. T. (2012). Rôle des reprises dans l’acquisition de la morpho-syntaxe chez des enfants francophones âgés de 2–3 ans. 12e Colloque de Logopédie “La parole reprise : formes, processus et fonctions”, Neuchâtel, Suisse.
Bloom, L. (1973). One Word at a Time . The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bloom. L., Hood, L., & Lightbown, P. (1974). Imitation in language development: If, when and why. Cognitive Psychology , 6, 380–420. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1968). The development of WH questions in child speech. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior , 7, 279–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., Cazden, C.B., & Bellugi, U. (1969). The child’s grammar from I to II. In J.P. Hill (Ed.), Minnesota symposia in child psychology , Vol. II (pp. 28–73). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. Google Scholar
Cazden, C.B. (1972). Child language and education . New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Chouinard, M.M., & Clark, E.V. (2003). Adult reformulations of child errors as negative evidence. Journal of Child Language , 30, 637–669. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V. (1999). Acquisition in the course of conversation. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences (Forum Lectures from the 1999 Linguistic Institute) 29(2), 1–18.Google Scholar
. (2004). Pragmatics and language acquisition. In L R. Horn & G. Ward (Eds.) Handbook of pragmatics (pp. 562–577). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
. (2009). First language acquisition, 2nd ed . Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V., & Bernicot, J. (2008). Repetition as ratification: How parents and children place information in common ground. Journal of Child Language , 35, 349–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V., & de Marneffe, M.-C. (2012). Constructing verb paradigms in French: Adult construals and emerging grammatical contrasts. Morphology , 22, 89–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H. (1996). Using Language . Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J. (1991). Understanding others: Evidence from naturalistic studies of children. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural Theories of Mind (pp. 51–61). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Dunn, J., & Munn, P. (1987). Development of justification in disputes with mother and sibling. Developmental Psychology , 23, 791–798. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R., & Wells, G. (1980). Enabling factors in child-adult discourse. First Language , 1, 46–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Farrar, M.J. (1992). Negative evidence and grammatical morpheme acquisition. Developmental Psychology , 28, 90–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Folger, J.P., & Chapman, R.S. (1978). A pragmatic analysis of spontaneous imitations. Journal of Child Language , 5, 25–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallaway, C., & Richards, B.J. (Eds.)(1994). Input and interaction in language acquisition . Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gampe, A., Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Eighteen-month-olds learn novel words through overhearing. First Language , 32, 385–397. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V.C.M., Sebastian, E. & Soto, P. (1999). The early acquisition of Spanish verbal morphology: Across-the-board or piecemeal knowledge? International Journal of Bilingualism , 3, 133–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, L.R., Newport, E.L., & Gleitman, H. (1984). The current status of the motherese hypothesis. Journal of Child Language , 11, 43–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goetz, P.J. (2010). The development of verbal justifications in the conversations of preschool children and adults. First Language , 30, 403–420. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R. (1986). ‘I beg your pardon?’: The preverbal negotiation of failed messages. Journal of Child Language , 13, 455–476. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gréco, P. (1985). Réduction et construction. Archives de Psychologie , 53, 21–35.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P., Reilly, J., Leaper, C., & Baker, N. (1985). The structural and functional status of single-word utterances and their relationship to early multi-word speech. In M.D. Barrett (Ed.), Children’s Single-Word Speech (pp. 233–267). Chichester: Wiley & Sons. Google Scholar
Gundel, J.K., Hedberg, N., & Zacharski, R. (1993). Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language , 69, 274–307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1985). Some contributions of mothers’ speech to their children’s syntactic growth. Journal of Child Language , 12, 367–385. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marcos, H. (1991). Reformulating requests at 18 months: Gestures, vocalisations and words. First Language , 11, 361–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M. (2006). The effect of perceptual availability and prior discourse on young children’s use of referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics , 27, 403–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moerk, E.L. (1991). Positive evidence for negative evidence. First Language , 11, 219–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laaha, S., Gillis, S., Kilani-Schoch, M., Korecky-Kröll, K., Xanthos, A., & Dressler, W.U. (2007). Weakly inflecting languages: French, Dutch, and German. In S. Laaha & S. Gillis (Eds.), Typological perspectives on the acquisition of noun and verb morphology (pp. 21–33). Antwerp: University of Antwerp.Google Scholar
Nelson, K.E. (1977). Facilitating children’s syntax acquisition. Developmental Psychology , 13, 101–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K.E., Baker, N.D., Denninger, M., Bonvillian, J.D. & Kaplan, B.J. (1985). Cookie versus do-it-again: Imitative-referential and personal-social syntactic-initiating language styles in young children. Linguistics , 23, 433–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K.E., Carskaddon, G., & Bonvillian, J. (1973). Syntax acquisition: Impact of experimental variation in adult verbal interaction with the child. Child Development , 44, 497–504. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Otomo, K. (2001). Maternal responses to word approximations in Japanese children’s transition to language. Journal of Child Language , 28, 29–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1964/1968). Genèse et structure en psychologie de l’intelligence. In D. Elkind (Ed.), Six études de psychologie (pp. 164–181). Genève: Gonthier (English Edition: Six Psychological Studies. New York, NY: Vintage Books, 1968).Google Scholar
. (1981/1987). Le possible et le nécessaire, Vol. 1: L’évolution des possibles chez l’enfant . Paris, France: P.U.F. (English Edition: Possibility and necessity, Vol.1: The role of possibility in cognitive development . Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press, 1987).Google Scholar
Piaget, J., & Garcia, R. (1983/1989). Psychogenèse et histoire des sciences . Paris: Flammarion. (English Psychogenesis and the history of science . New York, NY: Columbia University Press, 1989).Google Scholar
Ramer, A. (1976). The function of imitation in child language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research , 19, 700–717.Google Scholar
Rodgon, M., & Kurdek, L. (1977). Vocal and gestural imitation in children under 2 years old. Journal of Genetic Psychology , 131, 115–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saxton, M. (1997). The contrast theory of negative input. Journal of Child Language , 24, 139–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2000). Negative evidence and negative feedback: Immediate effects on the grammaticality of child speech. First Language , 20, 221–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schegloff, E.A. (1992). Repair after next turn: The last structurally provided defence of intersubjectivity in conversation. American Journal of Sociology , 97, 1295–1345. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scollon, R. (1979). A real early stage: An unzippered condensation of a dissertation on child language. In E. Ochs & B.B. Schieffelin (Eds.), Developmental pragmatics (pp. 215–277). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D.I. (1968). Imitation and grammatical development in children. In N. Endler, L. Boulter, & H. Osser (Eds.) Contemporary issues in developmental psychology (pp. 437–443). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Snow, C.E. (1987). Imitativeness: A trait or a skill? In G. Speidel & K.E. Nelson (Eds.), The many faces of imitation in language learning (pp. 73–90). New York NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Snow, C.E., & Ferguson, C.A. (Eds.) (1977). Talking to children: Language input and interaction . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Strapp, C.M. (1999). Mothers’, fathers’, and siblings’ responses to children’s language errors: Comparing sources of negative evidence. Journal of Child Language , 26, 373–391. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language , 4, 197–212. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E. (1984). Structural and formal precursors of language: An interactional approach . Unpublished doctoral dissertation. The Hebrew University, Jerusalem.Google Scholar
. (1988). Vocal-verbal interaction and the construction of early lexical knowledge. In M.D. Smith & J.L. Locke (Eds.) The emergent lexicon: The child’s development of a linguistic vocabulary (pp. 109–147). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
. (1997). Echanges conversationnels et acquisition première du langage. In J. Bernicot, A. Trognon & J. Caron-Pargue (Eds.), Conversation, interaction et fonctionnement cognitif (pp. 91–123). Nancy: Presses universitaires de Nancy.Google Scholar
. (1999). Early lexical, morphological and syntactic development in French: Some complex relations. International Journal of Bilingualism , 3, 183–217. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2001). Interactional processes in the origins of the explaining capacity. In K. Nelson, A. Aksu-Koç, & C. Johnson (Eds.), Children’s language, Vol. 10 (pp. 113–141). Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
. (2005). Effects of conversational functioning on early language acquisition: When both caregivers and children matter. In B. Bokus (Ed.), Studies in the Psychology of Child Language (pp. 47–69). Warsaw, Poland: Matrix.Google Scholar
. (2009). Processus inter et intra dans l’acquisition et le fonctionnement du langage. Cahiers du SRED , 15, 407–414.Google Scholar
. (2010). Conversation in language development and use: An introduction. First Language , 30(3–4), 241–249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2013). A cognitive-pragmatic model for the change from single-word to multiword speech: A constructivist approach. Journal of Pragmatics , 56, 133–150. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E., & Parisse, C. (2010). The acquisition of early verbs in French: Assessing the role of conversation and of child-directed speech. First Language , 30, 287–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E., & Sinclair, H. (1995). Functional changes in early child language: The appearance of references to the past and of explanations. Journal of Child Language , 22, 557–581. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2000). The changing status of “filler syllables” on the way to grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language , 27, 1–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E., Sinclair, H., & Berthoud, I. (1990). From one word to two words: Repetition patterns on the way to structured speech. Journal of Child Language , 17, 633–650. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wales Shugar, G. (1981). Early child discourse analyzed in the dyadic interaction unit. International Journal of Psycholinguistics 8(2), 55–78.Google Scholar
Cited by (4)

Cited by four other publications

Sudartinah, Titik
2023. A Conversation Analysis of Two-year-old Children’s Speech in Child-Adult Interactions. Jurnal Obsesi : Jurnal Pendidikan Anak Usia Dini 7:3  pp. 3345 ff. DOI logo
Lustigman, Lyle
2021. Early collaborations. Interactional Linguistics 1:2  pp. 216 ff. DOI logo
Lustigman, Lyle
2021. The emergence and development of complex predicates in early child Hebrew. First Language 41:4  pp. 430 ff. DOI logo
Salazar-Orvig, Anne, Geneviève de Weck, Rouba Hassan & Annie Rialland

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 28 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.