Part of
Language in Interaction: Studies in honor of Eve V. Clark
Edited by Inbal Arnon, Marisa Casillas, Chigusa Kurumada and Bruno Estigarribia
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 12] 2014
► pp. 231250
References (55)
References
Akhtar N., & Tomasello, M. (1997). Young children’s productivity with word order and verb morphology. Developmental Psychology , 33, 952–965. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ateş Şen B., Demir, Ö.D., & Küntay, A.C. (In preparation). Communicative actions and words in referential sets.
Avrutin, S., & Dina B. (2001). The expression of specificity in a language without determiners: Evidence from child Russian. In A.H.-J. Do, L. Domínguez, & A. Johansen (Eds.), Proceedings of 25th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 70–81). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1987). Competition, variation, and language learning. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.) Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp.157­–194), Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Batman-Ratyosyan, N., & Stromswold, K. (1999). What Turkish acquisition tells us about underlying word order and scrambling. Proceedings of the 23rd Annual University of Pennsylvania Linguistics Conference , 6, 37–52.
Batman-Ratyosyan, N., & Stromwold, K. (2002). Morphosyntax is easy, discourse pragmatics is hard. In A.H.-J. Do, L. Domínguez, & A. Johansen (Eds.), Proceedings of 25th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 793–804). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Candan, A., Küntay, A.C., Yeh, Y., Cheung, H., Wagner, L., & Naigles, L.R. (2012). Language and age effects in children’s processing of word order. Cognitive Development , 27, 205–221. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cannizzaro, C.L. (2012). Early word order and animacy . Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Groningen.Google Scholar
Casillas, M. (In press). Turn-taking. In D. Matthews (Ed.), Pragmatic Development in First Language . Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Chan, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Children’s understanding of the agent–patient relations in the transitive construction: Cross-linguistic comparisons between Cantonese, German and English. Cognitive Linguistics , 20, 267–300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chapman, R.S., & Miller, J.F. (1975). Word order in early two and three word utterances: Does production precede comprehension? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research , 18, 346–354.Google Scholar
Clark, E.V. (2012). An interview with our new President, Eve Clark / Interviewer: Angel Chan. IASCL Child Language Bulletin , 32.Google Scholar
Clark, E.V., & Bernicot, J. (2008). Repetition as ratification: How parents and children place information in common ground. Journal of Child Language , 35, 349–372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H. (1992). Arenas of language use . Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
. (1996). Using language . Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dasinger, L. (1995). The development of discourse competence in Finnish children: The expression of definiteness . Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Demir, Ö.E., So, W., Özyürek, A., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Turkish- and English-speaking children display sensitivity to perceptual context in the referring expressions they produce in speech and gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes , 27, 844–867. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Demiral, S.B., Schlesewsky, M., & Bornkessel-Schlesewsky I. (2008). On the universality of language comprehension strategies: Evidence from Turkish. Cognition , 106, 484–500. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dyakonova, M. (2004). Information structure development: Evidence from acquisition of word order in Russian and English. Nordlyd: Tromsø Working Papers , 32, 88–109.Google Scholar
Ekmekçi, F. (1979). The Effects of Simulation / Games on Foreign Language Learning . Unpublished MA thesis. The University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
. (1986). Significance of word order in the acquisition of Turkish. In D.I. Slobin & K. Zimmer (Eds.), Studies in Turkish linguistics (pp. 253–264). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ferreira, F., Anes, M.D., & Horine, M.D. (1996). Exploring the use of prosody during language comprehension using the auditory moving window technique. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research , 25, 273–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fisher, C., & Song, H. (2006). Who’s the subject? Sentence structures as analogs of verb meaning. In K. Hirsh-Pasek & Golinkoff, R.M. (Eds.), Action meets word: How children learn the meanings of verbs (pp. 392–428). Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J.A. (1983). Modularity of mind . Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gertner, Y., Fisher, C., & Eisengart, J. (2006). Learning words and rules: Abstract knowledge of word order in early sentence comprehension. Psychological Science , 17, 684–691. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gervain, J., Berent, I., & Werker, J.F. (2012). Binding at birth: The newborn brain detects identity relations and sequential position in speech. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 24, 564–574. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R.M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Cauley, K.M., & Gordon, L. (1987). The eyes have it: Lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm. Journal of Child Language , 14, 23–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Graf, E., & Davies, C. (In press). The development of reference. In D. Matthews (Ed.), Pragmatic Development in First Language . Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Gundel, J.K. (1988). Universals of topic-comment structure. In M. Hammond, E.A. Moravcssik, & J.R. Wirth (Eds.), Studies in syntactic typology (pp. 209–239). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hakuta, K. (1982). Interaction between particles and word order in the comprehension and production of simple sentences in Japanese children. Developmental Psychology , 18, 62–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, M., Hendriks, H., Roland, F., & Liang, J. (1996). The marking of new information in children’s narratives: A comparison of English, French, German and Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language , 23, 591–619. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ilic, T., & Deen, K.U. (2004). Object raising and cliticization in Serbo-Croatian child language. In the A. van Kampen & S. Baauw (Eds.), Proceedings of GALA 2003 (pp. 235–243). Utrecht: LOT.Google Scholar
Kamide, Y., Scheepers, C., & Altmann, G. (2003). Integration of syntactic and semantic information in predictive processing: Cross-linguistic evidence from German and English. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research , 32, 37–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Küntay, A.C. (2002). Development of the expression of indefiniteness: Presenting new referents in Turkish picture-series stories. Discourse Processes , 33, 77–101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Küntay, A., Nakamura, K., & Ateş Şen, B. (2014). Crosslinguistic and crosscultural approaches to pragmatic development. In D. Matthews (Ed.) Pragmatic Development in First Language . Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Küntay, A., & Slobin, D.I. (1996). Listening to a Turkish mother: Some puzzles for acquisition. In D. Slobin, A. Gerhardt, A. Kyratzis, & J. Guo (Eds.) Social interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 265–287). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associate.Google Scholar
Küntay, A.C., & Slobin, D.I. (2001). Discourse behavior of lexical categories in Turkish child-directed speech: Nouns vs. verbs. In M. Almgren, A. Barreña, M. Ezeizabarrena, I. Idiazabal, & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), Research on child language acquisition: Proceedings for the 8th Conference of the International Association for the Study of Child Language (pp. 928–946). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
. (2002). Putting interaction back into child language: Examples from Turkish. Psychology of Language and Communication , 6, 5–14.Google Scholar
Lambrecht, K. (1994). Information structure and sentence form: Topic, focus, and the mental representation of discourse referents . Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mykhaylyk, R. (2009). Developmental patterns in flexible word order acquisition. In J. Crawford, K. Otaki, & M. Takahashi (Eds.), Proceedings of the 3rd Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America (GALANA 2008) (pp. 165–174). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Naigles, L.R., & Swensen, L.D. (2006). Syntactic supports to word learning. In M. Shatz & E. Hoff (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of language development (pp. 212–231). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Otsu, Y. 1994. Early acquisition of scrambling in Japanese. In T. Hoekstra & B.D. Schwartz (Eds.), Language acquisition studies in generative grammar (pp. 253–264). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özge, D., Küntay, A.C. & Snedeker, J. (In preparation). Predictive processing of morphosyntax in Turkish-speaking children.
Özge D., Marinis, T., & Zeyrek D. (2013). Object-first orders in Turkish do not pose a challenge during processing. In U. Özge (Ed.), Proceedings of the 8th Workshop on Altaic Formal Languages (pp. 269–280). Cambridge, MA: MITWPL.Google Scholar
Sano, T. (2004). Scope relations of QP’s and scrambling in the acquisition of Japanese. In A. van Kampen & S. Baauw (Eds.), The Proceedings of GALA 2003 (pp. 421–431). Utrecht: LOT.Google Scholar
Slobin, D.I., & Bever, T.G. (1982). Children use canonical sentence schemas: A crosslinguistic study of word order and inflections. Cognition , 12, 229–265. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D.I., & Talay, A. (1986). Development of pragmatic uses of subject pronouns in Turkish child language. In A.A. Aksu-Koç & E. Erguvanlı Taylan (Eds.), Proceedings of the Turkish Linguistics Conference (pp. 207–228). Istanbul: Boğaziçi University Press.Google Scholar
Snow, C.E. (1995). Issues in the study of input: Fine-tuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes. In P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), Handbook of child language (pp. 180–193). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Song, H., & Fisher, C. (2007). Discourse prominence effects on 2.5-year-old children’s interpretation of pronouns. Lingua , 117, 1959–1987. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tannenhaus, M.K., & Trueswell, J.C. (2005). Eye movements as a tool for bridging the language-as-product and language-as-action traditions. In J.C. Trueswell & M.K. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Approaches to studying world-situated language use: Bridging the language-as-product and language-as-action traditions (pp. 3–37). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Trueswell J.C., & Tanenhaus, M.K. (2005). Approaches to studying world-situated language use: Bridging the language-as-product and language-as-action traditions . Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ural, A.E., Yüret, D., Ketrez, F.N., Koçbaş, D., & Küntay, A. (2009). Morphological cues vs. number of nominals in learning verb types in Turkish: The syntactic bootstrapping mechanism revisited. Language and Cognitive Processes , 24, 1393–1405. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yuan, S., Fisher, C., & Snedeker, J. (2012). Counting the nouns: Simple structural cues to verb meaning. Child Development , 83, 1382–1399. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yurovsky, D., Smith, L.B., & Yu, C. (2013). Statistical word learning at scale: The baby’s view is better. Developmental Science , 16(6), 959–966. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wittek, A., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Young children’s sensitivity to listener knowledge and perceptual context in choosing referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics , 26, 541–558. DOI logoGoogle Scholar