Part of
The Acquisition of Reference
Edited by Ludovica Serratrice and Shanley E.M. Allen
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 15] 2015
► pp. 241262
References (75)
References
Akhtar, N. (2005). The robustness of learning through overhearing. Developmental Science, 8, 199–209. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Akhtar, N., Jipson, J., & Callanan, M. (2001). Learning words through overhearing. Child Development, 72, 416–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anselmi, D., Tomasello, M., & Acunzo, M. (1986). Young children’s responses to neutral and specific contingent queries. Journal of Child Language, 13, 135–144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Asher, S.R., & Wigfield, A. (1981). Influence of comparison training on children’s referential communication. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 232–241. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ateş-Şen, A.B. (2010). The effects of discourse-pragmatic principles on young Turkish toddlers’ and their regular caregivers’ referential expressions (Unpublished MA thesis). Koç University, Turkey.Google Scholar
Ateş-Şen, A.B., Demir, Ö. E., & Küntay, A. (2011, April). Effects of discourse-pragmatic factors on verbal and gestural reference of Turkish toddlers and primary caregivers. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD) , Montreal, Canada.
Bahtiyar, S., & Küntay, A.C. (2009). Integration of communicative partner’s visual perspective in patterns of referential requests. Journal of Child Language, 36, 529–554. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, D.A. (1991). Infants’ contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development, 62, 875–890. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1993). Infants’ ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference. Journal of Child Language, 20, 395–418. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Banse, R., & Scherer, K.R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 614–636. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berman, J.M.J., Chambers, C.G., & Graham, S.A. (2010). Preschoolers’ appreciation of speaker vocal affect as a cue to referential intent. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 87–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berman, J.M.J., Graham, S.A., & Chambers, C.G. (2013). Contextual influences on children’s use of vocal affect cues during referential interpretation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 705–726. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D.V.M., & Adams, C. (1991). What do referential communication tasks measure? A study of children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 199–215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carmiol, A.M., & Vinden, P.G. (2013). Enhancing preschoolers’ understanding of ambiguity in communication: A training study on misunderstandings. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 59, 79–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, 1–174. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chow, V., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Lewis, J. (2008). To see or not to see: Infants prefer to follow the gaze of a reliable looker. Developmental Science, 11, 761–770. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clancy, P.M. (1986). The acquisition of Japanese. In D.I. Slobin (Ed.), The cross linguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 1 (pp. 373–524). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
. (1993). Preferred argument structure in Korean acquisition. In E.V. Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Child Language Research Forum. Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Colas, A. (1999). Introducing infants to referential events: A developmental study of maternal ostensive marking in French. Journal of Child Language, 26, 113–131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coon, R., Lipscomb, T., & Copple, C. (1982). Effects of listener feedback on the messages of kindergarten children in a referential communication task. The Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 3, 337–346. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, W., & Pechmann, T. (1982). Social interaction and the development of definite descriptions. Cognition, 11, 159–184. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dickson, W.P. (1982). Two decades of referential communication research: A review and meta-analysis. In C.J. Brainerd & M. Pressley (Eds.), Verbal processes in children (pp. 1–33). New York, NY: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Estigarribia, B., & Clark, E.V. (2007). Getting and maintaining attention in talk to young children. Journal of Child Language, 34, 799–814. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fagan, M.K. (2008). Toddlers’ persistence when communication fails: Response motivation and goal substitution. First Language, 28, 55–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Floor, P., & Akhtar, N. (2006). Can 18-month-old infants learn words by listening in on conversations? Infancy, 9, 327–339. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frank, M.C., Tenenbaum, J.B., & Fernald, A. (2013). Social and discourse contributions to the determination of reference in cross-situational word learning. Language Learning and Development, 9, 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, T. (1977). Revision behaviors in the speech of normal children developing language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 20, 303–318. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glucksberg, S., & Krauss, R.M. (1967). What do people say after they have learned how to talk? Studies of the development of referential communication. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 13, 309–316.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R.M. (1986). ‘‘I beg your pardon?’’: The preverbal negotiation of failed messages. Journal of Child Language, 13, 455–476. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grassman, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young children follow pointing over words in interpreting acts of reference. Developmental Science, 13, 252–263. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guerriero, A.M.S., Oshima-Takane, Y., & Kuriyama Y. (2006). The development of referential choice in English and Japanese: A discourse-pragmatic perspective. Journal of Child Language, 33, 823–857. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huang, C. (2012). Referential choice and informativeness in mother-child conversation: A focus on the mother. Language Sciences, 34, 319–338. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hughes, M.E., & Allen, S.E.M. (2013). The effect of individual discourse-pragmatic features on referential choice in child English. Journal of Pragmatics, 56, 15–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2015). The incremental effect of discourse-pragmatic sensitivity on referential choice in the acquisition of a first language. Lingua, 155, 43–61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J.M., Capirci, O., Longobardi, E., & Caselli, C.M. (1999). Gesturing in mother-child interactions. Cognitive Development, 14, 57–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kidd, C., White, K.S., & Aslin, R.N. (2011). Toddlers use speech disfluencies to predict speakers’ referential intentions. Developmental Science, 14, 925–934. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krogh-Jespersen, S., & Echols, C.H. (2012). The influence of speaker reliability on first versus second label learning. Child Development, 83, 581–590.Google Scholar
Küntay, A., Nakamura, K., & Ateş-Şen, A.B. (2014). Crosslinguistic and crosscultural approaches to pragmatic development. In D. Matthews (Ed.), Pragmatic development in first language acquisition (pp. 317–341). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leekam, S.R., Solomon, T.L., & Teoh, Y. (2010). Adults’ social cues facilitate young children’s use of signs and symbols. Developmental Science, 13, 108–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lefebvre-Pinard, M., & Reid, L. (1980). A comparison of three methods of training communication skills: Social conflict, modeling, and conflict-modeling. Child Development, 51, 179–187. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Henning, A., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science, 7, 297–307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12 months of age. Developmental Science, 10, 1–7. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2008). Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition, 108, 732–739. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Butcher, J., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Two- and four-year-olds learn to adapt referring expressions to context: Effects of distracters and feedback on referential communication. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4, 184–210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2007). How toddlers and preschoolers learn to uniquely identify referents for others: A training study. Child Development, 78, 1744–1759. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., Angelopoulos, M., & Bennett, P. (1999). Word learning in the context of referential and salience cues. Developmental Psychology, 35, 60–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morisseau, T., Davies, C., & Matthews, D. (2013). How do 3- and 5-year-olds respond to over- and under-informative utterances? Journal of Pragmatics, 59A, 26–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Namy, L.L., & Nolan, S.A. (2004). Characterizing changes in parent labeling and gesturing and their relation to early communicative development. Journal of Child Language, 31, 821–835. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Narasimhan, B., Budwig, N., & Murty, L. (2005). Argument realization in Hindi caregiver-child discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 461–495. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nilsen, E.S., Graham, S.A., & Pettigrew,T. (2009). Preschoolers’ word mapping: The interplay between labeling context and specificity of speaker information. Journal of Child Language, 36, 673–684. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nilsen, E.S., & Mangal, L. (2012). Which is important for preschoolers’ production and repair of statements: What the listener knows or what the listener says? Journal of Child Language, 39, 1121–1134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Neill, M., Bard, K.A., Linell, M., & Fluck, M. (2005). Maternal gestures with 20-month-old infants in two contexts. Developmental Science, 8, 352–359. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Neill, D.K., & Topolovec, J. (2001). Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to the referential(in)efficacy of their own pointing gestures. Journal of Child Language, 28, 1–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oshima-Takane, Y., Goodz, E., & Derevensky, J.L. (1996). Birth order effects on early language development: Do second born children learn from overheard speech? Child Development, 67, 621–634. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Navarro, S. (2003). Subject realization and crosslinguistic interference in the bilingual acquisition of Spanish and English: What is the role of input? Journal of Child Language, 30, 371–393. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peterson, D., Danner, F., & Flavell, J. (1972). Developmental changes in children’s response to three indications of communicative failure. Child Development, 43, 1463–1468. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rader, N.V., & Zukow-Goldring, P. (2012). Caregivers’ gestures direct infant attention during early word learning: The importance of dynamic synchrony. Language Sciences, 34, 559–568. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Robinson, E.J., & Robinson, W.P. (1981). Ways of reacting to communication failure in relation to the development of children’s understanding about verbal communication. European Journal of Social Psychology, 11, 189–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1985). Teaching children about verbal referential communication. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 8, 285–299. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rohlfing, K.J., Longo, M.R., & Bertenthal, B.I. (2012). Dynamic pointing triggers shifts of visual attention in young infants. Developmental Science, 15, 426–435. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rozendaal, M., & Baker, A. (2008). A cross-linguistic investigation of the acquisition of the pragmatics of indefinite and definite reference in two-year-olds. Journal of Child Language, 38, 1–35.Google Scholar
. (2010). The acquisition of reference: Pragmatic aspects and the influences of language input. Journal of Pragmatics, 42, 1866–1879. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruffman, T., Olson, D.R., & Astington, J.W. (1991). Children’s understanding of visual ambiguity. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 89–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sarılar, A., Matthews, D. & Küntay, A. (2015). Hearing relative clauses boosts relative clause usage (and referential clarity) in young Turkish language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 175–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L. (2008). The role of discourse and perceptual cues in the choice of referential expressions in English preschoolers, school-age children, and adults. Language Learning and Development, 4, 309–332. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shneidman, L.A., Buresh, J.S., Shimpi, P.M., Knight-Schwarz, J., & Woodward, A.L. (2009). Social experience, social attention and word learning in an overhearing paradigm. Language Learning and Developmen, 5, 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D.I. (1986). The acquisition and use of relative clauses in Turkic and Indo-European languages. In D.I. Slobin & K. Zimmer (Eds.), Studies in Turkish linguistics (pp. 273–291). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
So, W.C., & Lim, J.Y. (2012). Point to a referent, and say, “what is this?” Gesture as a potential cue to identify referents in a discourse. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33, 329–342. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sonnenschein, S., & Whitehurst, G. (1984). Developing referential communication: A hierarchy of skills. Child Development, 55, 1936–1945. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Varghese, A.L., & Nilsen, E.S. (2013). Incentives improve the clarity of school-age children’s referential statements. Cognitive Development, 28, 364–373. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G. (1976). The development of communication: Changes with age and modeling. Child Development, 47, 473–482. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whitehurst, G.J., Sonnenschein, S., & Ianfolla, B.J. (1981). Learning to communicate from models: Children confuse length with information. Child Development, 52, 507–513. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, M.J., & Webster, E.J. (1980). Early discourse behavior: An analysis of children’s responses to listener feedback. Child Development, 51, 1120–1125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zukow-Goldring, P. (1996). Sensitive caregiving fosters the comprehension of speech: When gestures speak louder than words. Early Development and Parenting, 5, 195–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (12)

Cited by 12 other publications

Vasil, Jared
2023. A New Look at Young Children’s Referential Informativeness. Perspectives on Psychological Science 18:3  pp. 624 ff. DOI logo
Bacso, Sarah A. & Elizabeth S. Nilsen
2022. Children’s use of verbal and nonverbal feedback during communicative repair: Associations with executive functioning and emotion knowledge. Cognitive Development 63  pp. 101199 ff. DOI logo
Peeters, David, Emiel Krahmer & Alfons Maes
2021. A conceptual framework for the study of demonstrative reference. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 28:2  pp. 409 ff. DOI logo
Rojas-Nieto, Cecilia
2021. Interactional sequences: child–adult collaboration in solving referring problems (Secuencias interlocutivas: colaboración adulto-niño en la solución de problemas referenciales). Journal for the Study of Education and Development 44:2  pp. 439 ff. DOI logo
Sadiq, Raghad Baker, Nadire Cavus & Dogan Ibrahim
2021. Mobile application based on CCI standards to help children learn English as a foreign language. Interactive Learning Environments 29:3  pp. 442 ff. DOI logo
Salazar-Orvig, Anne, Geneviève de Weck, Rouba Hassan & Annie Rialland
Salazar-Orvig, Anne, Geneviève de Weck, Rouba Hassan & Annie Rialland
ATEŞ, BEYZA Ş. & AYLİN C. KÜNTAY
2018. Referential interactions of Turkish-learning children with their caregivers about non-absent objects: integration of non-verbal devices and prior discourse. Journal of Child Language 45:1  pp. 148 ff. DOI logo
Belpaeme, Tony, Paul Vogt, Rianne van den Berghe, Kirsten Bergmann, Tilbe Göksun, Mirjam de Haas, Junko Kanero, James Kennedy, Aylin C. Küntay, Ora Oudgenoeg-Paz, Fotios Papadopoulos, Thorsten Schodde, Josje Verhagen, Christopher D. Wallbridge, Bram Willemsen, Jan de Wit, Vasfiye Geçkin, Laura Hoffmann, Stefan Kopp, Emiel Krahmer, Ezgi Mamus, Jean-Marc Montanier, Cansu Oranç & Amit Kumar Pandey
2018. Guidelines for Designing Social Robots as Second Language Tutors. International Journal of Social Robotics 10:3  pp. 325 ff. DOI logo
CARMIOL, Ana M., Danielle MATTHEWS & Odir A. RODRÍGUEZ-VILLAGRA
2018. How children learn to produce appropriate referring expressions in narratives: the role of clarification requests and modeling. Journal of Child Language 45:3  pp. 736 ff. DOI logo
Heller, Vivien & Katharina J. Rohlfing
2017. Reference as an Interactive Achievement: Sequential and Longitudinal Analyses of Labeling Interactions in Shared Book Reading and Free Play. Frontiers in Psychology 8 DOI logo
Vogt, Paul, Mirjam de Haas, Chiara de Jong, Peta Baxter & Emiel Krahmer
2017. Child-Robot Interactions for Second Language Tutoring to Preschool Children. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 7 november 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.