Part of
The Acquisition of Turkish in Childhood
Edited by Belma Haznedar and F. Nihan Ketrez
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 20] 2016
► pp. 119150
References (45)
References
Ambridge, B. & Lieven, E.V.M. 2011. Child Language Acquisition: Contrasting Theoretical Approaches. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Amidon, A. & Carey, P. 1972. Why five-year-olds cannot understand before and after . Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 11: 417–423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barrie-Blackley, S. 1973. Six-year-old children’s understanding of sentences adjoined with time adverbs. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 2: 153–165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berwick, R. 1985. Acquisition of Syntactic Knowledge. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1981. Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
1995. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E.V. 1971. On the acquisition of the meaning of before and after . Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 10: 266–275. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crain, S. 1982. Temporal terms: Mastery by age five. In Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, Proceedings of the Fourteenth Annual Stanford Child Language Research Forum 21: 33–38. Stanford University.Google Scholar
2008. The interpretation of disjunction in Universal Grammar. Language and Speech 5: 151–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2012. Emergence of Meaning. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crain, S. Gardner, A., Gualmini, A. & Rabbin, B. 2002. Children’s command of negation. In Proceedings of the 3rd Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics, Y. Otsu (eds), 71–95. Tokyo: Hituzi.Google Scholar
Crain, S., Goro, T. & Thornton, R. 2006. Language acquisition is language change. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 35: 31–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crain, S., Goro, T., Notley, A. & Zhou, P. 2013. A parametric account of scope in child language. In Advances in Language Acquisition, S. Stavrakaki, M. Lalioti & P. Konstantinopoulou (eds), 63–71. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Crain, S., Gualmini, A. & Pietroski, P. 2005. Brass tracks in linguistic theory: innate grammatical principles. In The Innate Mind: Structure and Contents, P. Carruthers, S. Laurence & S. Stitch (eds.), 175–197. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crain, S. & Khlentzos, D. 2008. Is logic innate? Biolinguistics 2(1): 24–56.Google Scholar
Crain, S., Ni, W. & Conway, L. 1994. Learning, parsing and modularity. In Perspectives on Sentence Processing, C. Clifton Jr., L. Frazier & K. Rayner (eds.), 443–467. Hilldale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Crain, S. & Thornton, R. 1998. Investigations in Universal Grammar: A Guide to Experiments on the Acquisition of Syntax and Semantics. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Epstein, S., Flynn, S. & Martohardjono, G. 1996. Second language acquisition: Theoretical and experimental issues in contemporary research. Brain and Behavioral Sciences 19: 677–758. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreiro, E. & Sinclair, H. 1971. Temporal relations in language. International Journal of Psychology 6: 39–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A.E. 2003. Constructions: A new theoretical approach to language. Trends in Cognitive Science 7(5): 219–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goro, T. 2004. The Emergence of Universal Grammar in the Emergence of Language: The Acquisition of Japanese Logical Connectives and Positive Polarity. MA dissertation, University of Maryland.Google Scholar
2007. Language-specific Constraints on Scope Interpretation in First Language Acquisition. PhD dissertation, University of Maryland.Google Scholar
Goro, T. & Akiba, S. 2004a. The acquisition of disjunction and positive polarity in Japanese. In WCCFL 23 Proceedings of the 23rd West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, V. Chand, A. Kelleher, A. J. Rodriguez & B. Schmeiser (eds), 251–264. Somerville MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
2004b. Japanese disjunction and the acquisition of positive polarity. In Proceedings of the Fifth Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics, Y. Otsu (ed.), 137–162. Tokyo: Hituzi Shobo.Google Scholar
Gorrell, P., Crain, S. & Fodor, J. 1989. Contextual information and temporal terms. Journal of Child Language 16: 623–632. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Göksel, A. & Kerslake, C. 2005. Turkish: A Comprehensive Grammar. London: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grice, H.P. 1975. Logic and conversation. In Syntax and Semantics, Vol. 3: Speech Acts, P. Cole & J. Morgan (eds.), 41–58. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Grüter, T., Lieberman, M. & Gualmini, A. 2010. Acquiring the scope of disjunction and negation in L2: A bidirectional study of learners of Japanese and English. Language Acquisition 17(3): 127–154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gualmini, A. & Crain, S. 2002. Why no child or adult must learn De Morgan’s laws. In BUCLD 26: Proceedings of the 26th Boston University Conference on Language Development, B. Skarabela, S. Fish & A. H-J. Do (eds), 243–254. Somerville MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T., Chierchia, G., Crain, S., Foppolo, F., Gualmini, A. & Meroni, L. 2005. Why children and adults sometimes (but not always) compute implicatures. Language and Cognitive Processes 20(5): 667–696. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hatch, E. 1971. The young child’s comprehension of time connectives. Child Development 42: 211–213. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haznedar, B. & Schwartz, B. D. 1997. Are there optional infinitives in child L2 acquisition? In BUCLD 21: Proceedings of the 21st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Acquisition, E. Hughes, M. Hughes & A. Greenhill (eds), 231–239. Somerville MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Lee, O.S. 2010. Acquisition of Korean disjunction under negation. University of Hawaii Working Papers in Linguistics 41(7): 1–12.Google Scholar
Lieven, E.V.M. & Tomasello, M. 2008. Children’s first language acquisition from a usage-based perspective. In Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition, P. Robinson & N. Ellis (eds), 168–196. New York NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Morris, B. 2008. Logically speaking: Evidence for item-based acquisition of the connectives AND and OR. Journal of Cognition and Development 9(1): 67–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Notley, A., Zhou, P., Jensen, B. & Crain, S. 2012. Children’s interpretation of disjunction in the scope of ‘before’: A comparison of English and Mandarin. Journal of Child Language 39(3): 482–522. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Notley, A., Thornton, R. & Crain, S. 2013. English-speaking children’s interpretation of disjunction in the scope of ‘not every’. Biolinguistics 6(1): 32–69.Google Scholar
Noveck, I.A. 2001. When children are more logical than adults: Investigations of scalar implicature. Cognition 78(2): 165–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. 1984. Language Learnability and Language Development. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B.D. & Sprouse, R. 1994. Word order and nominative case in non-native language acquisition: A longitudinal study of (L1 Turkish) German interlanguage. In Language Acquisition Studies in Generative Grammar [Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 8], T. Hoekstra & B.D. Schwartz (eds), 317–368. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Su, Y. & Crain, S. 2013. Children’s knowledge of disjunction and universal quantification in Mandarin Chinese. Language and Linguistics 14(3): 599–631.Google Scholar
Szabolcsi, A. 2002. Hungarian disjunctions and positive polarity. In Approaches to Hungarian 8, I. Kenesei & P. Siptar (eds), 217–241. Budapest: Akademiai Kiado.Google Scholar
Thornton, R. & Crain, S. 2013. Parameters: The pluses and the minuses. In The Cambridge Handbook of Generative Syntax, M. den Dikken (ed), 927–970. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. 2000. First steps toward a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cognitive Linguistics 11(1): 61–82.Google Scholar
2003. Constructing a Language: A Usage-based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar