Part of
The Acquisition of Turkish in Childhood
Edited by Belma Haznedar and F. Nihan Ketrez
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 20] 2016
► pp. 325340
References (65)
References
Acarlar, F., & Johnston, J.R. 2011. Acquisition of Turkish grammatical morphology by children with developmental disorders. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 46(6): 728–738. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aksu-Koç, A. 1996. Frames of mind through narrative discourse. In Social Interaction, Social Context, and Language: Essays in Honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp, D.I. Slobin, J. Gerhardt, A., Kyratsiz & J. Guo (eds), 309–328. Hillsdale NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Aksu-Koç, A. & Slobin, D.I. 1985. The acquisition of Turkish. In The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition, Vol. 1: The Data, D.I. Slobin (ed.), 839–878. Hillsdale NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Aksu-Koç, A. & Tekdemir, G. 2004. Interplay between narrativity and mindreading: Comparison between Turkish and English. In Relating Events in Narrative: Typological and Contextual Perspectives, S. Stromquist & L. Verhoeven (eds), 307–327. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Alev, G., Diken, İ.H., Ardıç, A, Diken, Ö, Şekercioğlu, G. & Gilliam, J.E. 2014. Adaptation and examining psychometrical properties of pragmatic language skills inventory (PLSI) in Turkey. Elementary Education Online 13(1): 258–273.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-V. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A.M. & Frith, U. 1985. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 21: 37–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Moriarty, J., Schmitz, B., Costa, D. & Ell, P. 1994. Recognition of mental state terms. Clinical findings in children with autism and a functional neuroimaging study of normal adults. British Journal of Psychiatry 165(5): 640–649. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bartolucci, G., Pierce, S.J. & Streiner, D. 1980. Cross-sectional studies of grammatical morphemes in autistic and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 10(1): 39–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Marchman, V.A., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J.S. et al. 1994. Development and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 21: 85–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berument, S.K., & Güven, A.G. 2010. TİFALDİ Alıcı Dil Kelime Alt Testi Kullanım Klavuzu (Users’ Manual for Receptive Vocabulary Sub-test). Ankara: Kültür ve Turizm Bakanlığı.Google Scholar
Boucher, J. 2012. Research review: Structural language in autistic spectrum disorder: characteristics and causes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 53(3): 219–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. 1973. A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S. & van der Lely, H.K.J. 2008. Narrative discourse in adults with high-functioning autism or asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 38: 28–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dale, P.S. & Crain-Thoreson, C. 1993. Pronoun reversals: Who, when, and why? Journal of Child Language 20(3): 573–589. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
deVilliers, J.G. 2000. Language and theory of mind: What are the developmental relationships? In Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism and Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg & D. Cohen (eds), 83–123. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Diken, Ö. 2014. Pragmatic language skills of children with developmental disabilities: A descriptive and relational study in Turkey. Eurasian Journal of Educational Research 55: 109–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diken, I.H., Ardic, A., Diken, Ö. & Gilliam, J.E. 2012. Exploring validity and reliability of Turkish Version of Gilliam Autism Rating Scale-2. Education and Science 37(166): 318–328.Google Scholar
Dunn, M., Gomes, H. & Sebastian, M. 1996. Prototypicality of responses in autistic language disordered and normal children in a verbal fluency task. Child Neuropsychology 2: 99–108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eigsti, I.M., Bennetto, L. & Dadlani, M.B. 2007. Beyond pragmatics: Morphosyntactic development in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37(6): 1007–1023. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eigsti, I.M., de Marchena, A.B., Schuh, A.B. & Kelley, E. 2011. Language acquisition in autism spectrum disorders: A developmental review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5(2): 681–691. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S., Gernsbacher, M., Stronach, S., Karasinski, C., Eernisse, E., Erickson, C. & Sindberg, H. 2011. Lexical and grammatical skills in toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to late talking toddlers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 41: 1065–1075. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Emerich, D.M., Creaghead, N.A., Grether, S.M., Murray, D. & Grasha, C. 2003. The comprehension of humorous materials by adolescents with high-functioning autism and Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 33(3): 253–257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fein, D., Dunn, M., Allen, D., Aram, D., Hall, N., Morris, R. & Wilson, B. 1996. Language and neuropsychological findings. In Preschool Children with Inadequate Communication, I. Rapin (ed.), 123–154. London: MacKeith Press.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Bates, E., Thal, D. & Pethick, S. 1994. Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59: 14–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gilliam, J.E. & Miller, L. 2006. Pragmatic Language Skills Inventory (PLSI). Austin TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Güven, A.G. & Berument, S.K. 2010. TİFALDİ İfade Edici Dil Kelime Alt Testi Kullanım Klavuzu (Users’ Manual for Expressive Vocabulary Sub-Test). Ankara: Kültür ve Turizm Bakanlığı.Google Scholar
Hale, C.M. & Tager-Flusberg, H. 2005. Social communication in children with autism: The relationship between theory of mind and discourse development. Autism 9(2): 157–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Hobson, J. 2010. Personal pronouns and communicative engagement in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 40(6): 653–664. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hudry, K., Leadbitter, K., Temple, K., Slonims, V., McConachie, H. & Aldred, C. 2010. Preschoolers with autism show greater impairment in receptive compared to expressive language abilities. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 45: 681–690. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher J. 1974. The origins of language comprehension. In Theories of Cognitive Psychology, R. Solso (ed.), 331–388. New York NY: Halsted Press.Google Scholar
Jarrold, C., Boucher, J. & Russell, J. 1997. Language profiles in children with autism: theoretical and methodological implications. Autism 1: 57–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Joanisse, M.F. & Seidenberg, M.S. 1998. Specific language impairment: A deficit in grammar or processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2: 240–247 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelley, E., Paul, J., Fein, D. & Naigles, L. 2006. Residual language deficits in optimal outcome children with a history of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 36(6): 807–828. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kjelgaard, M.M. & Tager-Flusberg, H. 2001. An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes 16: 287–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Klinger, L., Dawson, G. & Renner, P. 2002. Autistic disorder. In Child Psychopathology, E. Mash & R. Barkley (eds), 409–454. New York NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Krug, D.A., Arick, J.R. & Almond, P.A. 1993. Autism Screening Instrument for Educational Planning, 2nd edn. Austin TX: Pro-ed.Google Scholar
Küntay, A.C. & Slobin, D.I. 2001. Discourse behavior of lexical categories in Turkish child-directed speech: Nouns vs. verbs. In Research on Child Language Acquisition: Proceedings for the 8th Conference of the International Association for the study of Child Language, M. Almgren, A. Barrena, M. Ezeizabarrena, I. Idiazabal & B. MacWhinney (eds), 928–946. Boston MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Lee, A., Hobson, R.P. & Chiat, S. 1994. I, you, me and autism: An experimental study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24: 155–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loveland, K. & Tunali, B. 1993. Narrative language in autism and the theory of mind hypothesis: A wider perspective. In Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism, S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg & D. J. Cohen (eds), 247–266. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Loveland, K., McEvoy, R., Tunali, B. & Kelley, M.L. 1990. Narrative story telling in autism and Down’s syndrome. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 8: 9–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Losh, M. & Capps, L. 2003. Narrative ability in high-functioning children with autism or Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 33(3): 239–251.Google Scholar
Luyster, R., Lopez, K. & Lord, C. 2007. Characterizing communicative development in children referred for autism spectrum disorders using the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (CDI). Journal of Child Language 34: 623–654. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Naigles, L., Kelty, E., Jaffery, R. & Fein, D. 2011. Abstractness and continuity in the syntactic development of young children with autism. Autism Research 4: 422–437. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özcan, M. 2007. Developmental differences in the use of tense aspect modality in narratives. Hacettepe Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi 32: 221–231.Google Scholar
Park, C. J., Yelland, G. W., Taffe, J. R. & Gray, K. M. 2012. Morphological and syntactic skills in language samples of pre-school aged children with autism: atypical development? International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 14(2): 95–108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perkins, M., Dobbinson, S., Boucher, J., Bol, S. & Bloom, P. 2006. Lexical knowledge and lexical use in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 36: 795–805. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L. & Safyer, P. 2013. Lexical composition in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Language 40: 47–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scarborough, H. 1990. Index of productive syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics 11: 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. & Aksu, A. 1982. Tense, aspect, and modality in the use of the Turkish evidential. In Tense-Aspect: Between Semantics and Pragmatics [Typological Studies in Language 1], P. Hopper (ed.),185–200. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. 2000. Learning how to learn words: An associative crane. In Becoming a Word Learner: A Debate on Lexical Acquisition, R.M. Golinkoff, R.M. Golinkoff, K. Hirsh-Pasek, L. Bloom, L.B. Smith, A.L. Woodard, N. Akhtar, M. Tomasello & G. Hollich (eds), 51–80. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Surian, L. & Leslie, A. 1999. Competence and performance in false belief understanding: A comparison of autistic and three-year-old children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 17: 131–145. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. 2004. Strategies for conducting research on language in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34: 75–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2006. Defining language phenotypes in autism. Clinical Neuroscience Research 6: 219–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., Calkins, S., Nolin, T., Baumberger, T., Anderson, M. & Chadwick-Dias, A. 1990. A longitudinal study of language acquisition in autistic and Down syndrome children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 20: 1–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., Paul, R. & Lord, C. 2005. Communication in autism. In Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, 3rd edn, F. Volkmar, A. Klin, R. Paul & D. cohen (eds), 335-364. New York NY: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. & Sullivan, K. 1995. Attributing mental states to story characters: A comparison of narratives produced by autistic and mentally retarded individuals. Applied Psycholinguistics 16: 241–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tek, S., Jaffery, G., Fein, D. & Naigles, L. R. 2008. Do children with autism spectrum disorders show a shape bias in word learning? Autism Research 1(4): 208–222. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tek, S., Mesite, L., Fein, D. & Naigles, L. 2014. Longitudinal analyses of expressive language development reveal two distinct language profiles among young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 44: 75–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tekinsav-Sütcü, S., Aydın, A. Yılmaz-Irmak, T., Mortan, O., Sevi, S., Özmen, Z. & Şengün E. (2008). Otizmde dil ve iletişim özelliklerinin incelenmesi (Language and communication characteristics in Autism). Çocuk ve Gençlik Ruh Sağlığı Dergisi 15(3): 139–148.Google Scholar
Volden, J. & Lord, C. 1991. Neologisms and idiosyncratic language in autistic speakers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 21(2): 109–130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Volden, J., Coolican, J., Garon, N., White, J. & Bryson, S. 2009. Brief report: Pragmatic language in autism spectrum disorder: Relationship to measures of ability and disability. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 39: 388–393. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Walenski, M., Mostofsky, S.H. & Ullman, M.T. 2014. Inflectional morphology in high-functioning autism: Evidence for speeded grammatical processing. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 8: 1607–1621. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, D.M., & Happe, F. 2009. “What did I say?” versus “What did I think?” Attributing false beliefs to self amongst children with and without autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 39: 865–873. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, D., Botting, N. & Boucher, J., 2008. Language in autism and specific language impairment: Where are the links? Psychological Bulletin 134: 944–963. DOI logoGoogle Scholar