Part of
Sources of Variation in First Language Acquisition: Languages, contexts, and learners
Edited by Maya Hickmann †, Edy Veneziano and Harriet Jisa
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 22] 2018
► pp. 339364
References (61)
References
Acredolo, L. & Goodwyn S. 1988. Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development 59(2): 450–466. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, D. & Reilly, J. 1997. The puzzle of negation : How children move from communicative to grammatical negation in ASL., Journal of Child Language 18(4): 411–429.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. 2012. How the Brain Got Language: The Mirror System Hypothesis. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. 1979. The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in Infancy. New York NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Beaupoil-Hourdel, P. 2015. Acquisition et expression multimodale de la négation. Etude d’un corpus vidéo et longitudinal de dyades mère-enfant francophone et anglophone. PhD dissertation, Université Sorbonne Nouvelle – Paris 3.Google Scholar
Beaupoil-Hourdel, P., Boutet, D. & Morgenstern, A. 2015. A child’s multimodal negations from 1 to 4: The interplay between modalities. In Negation and Polarity: Experimental Perspectives, Language, Cognition, and Mind, P. Larrivée & C. Lee (eds), 95–123. Bern: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bellugi, U. 1967. A Transformational Analysis of the Development of Negation. PhD dissertation, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Benazzo, S. & Morgenstern, A. 2014. A bilingual child’s multimodal path into negation. Gesture 14(2): 171–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blondel, M. 2009. Acquisition bilingue LSF-français. L'enfant qui grandit avec deux langues et dans deux modalités. In Au croisement de différents types d'acquisition: Pourquoi et comment comparer?, S. Benazzo (ed.). AILE…LIA 1: 169–194.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. 1970. Language Development: Form and Function in Emerging Grammars. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
1991. Language Development from Two to Three. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Butcher, C. & Goldin-Meadow, S. 2000. Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: When hand and mouth come together. In Language and Gesture, D. McNeill (ed.), 235–257. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caët, S. 2013. Référence à soi et à l’interlocuteur chez des enfants francophones et anglophones et leurs parents. PhD dissertation, Université Sorbonne Nouvelle – Paris 3.Google Scholar
Calbris, G. 2005. La négation: Son symbolisme physique. Interacting Bodies – Corps en Interaction. Second Conference of the International Society for Gesture Studies, Lyon: France.Google Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E. & Theakston, A. L. 2007. What part of no do children not understand? A usage-based account of multiword negation. Journal of Child Language 33: 251–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capirci O., Iverson, J. M., Pizzuto, E. & Volterra, V. 1996. Gestures and words during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child Language 23(03): 645–673. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Choi, S. 1988. The semantic development of negation: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Child Language 15: 517–532. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. 1978. From gesture to word, on the natural history of deixis in language acquisition. In Human Growth and Development: Wolfson College Lectures 1976, J. S. Bruner & A. Garton (eds), 85–120. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Clark, E. V. 1977. Psychology of Language: An Introduction to Psycholinguistics. New York NY: Harcourt, Brace.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. 1872. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: John Murray. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dodane, C. & Massini-Cagliari, G. 2010. La prosodie dans l’acquisition de la négation : étude de cas d’une enfant monolingue française. ALFA: Revista de Linguística, 54(2).Google Scholar
Ekman, P. & Friesen, W.V. 1969. The repertoire or nonverbal behavior: categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica 1: 49–98.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K. 2002. Language, Cognition, and the Brain: Insights from SIGN Language Research. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Guidetti, M. 2005. Yes or no? How do young children combine gestures and words to agree and refuse? Journal of child language 32: 911–924. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harrison, S. 2010. Evidence for node and scope of negation in coverbal gesture. Gesture 10(1): 29–51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horn, L. R. 2001. A Natural History of Negation. Stanford CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Kanto, L., Lassako, M., Huttunen, K. 2015. Differentiation in language and gesture use during early bilingual development of hearing children of deaf parents. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18(4): 769–788. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. 1988. How gestures can become like words. In Cross-cultural Perspectives in Nonverbal Communication, F. Poyatos (ed.), 131–141. Lewiston NY: C.J. Hogrefe.Google Scholar
2002. Some uses of the headshake. Gesture 2(2): 147–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2004. Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kita, S. & Özyürek, A. 2003. What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language 48: 16–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Limousin, F. 2011. Acquisition de la reference personnelle en LSF: Analyse longitudinale des pointages, des forms nulls et des noms signés chez une enfant soured de parents sourds. PhD dissertation, University of Paris 8.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. 2000. The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk. Transcription format and programs. Vol. 1. Routledge.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. & Nicoladis, E. 2000. Gesture reflects language development: Evidence from bilingual children. Psychological science 9(6): 192–196.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. 1992. Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal about Thought. Chicago IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. & McNeill. N. B. 1968. What does a child mean when he says no? In Proceedings of the Conference on Language and Language Behaviour, E. M. Zale (ed.), 464–473. New York NY: Appleton Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, A. 2006. Un JE en construction. Paris: Presses de la Sorbonne Nouvelle.Google Scholar
2009. L’enfant dans la langue. Paris: Presses de la Sorbonne Nouvelle.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, A. & Beaupoil, P. 2015. Multimodal approaches to language acquisition through the lens of negation. Vestnik of Moscow State University. Linguistics and Literary Studies 6(717): 435–451. Special issue on Discourse as Social Practice.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, A., Beaupoil, P., Blondel, M. & Boutet, D. 2016. A multimodal approach to the development of negation in signed and spoken languages: Four case studies. In The Usage-Based Study of Language Learning and Multilingualism, L. Ortega, A. E. Tyler, H. I. Park & M. Uno (eds), 15–36. Washington DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, A. & Parisse, C. 2012. The Paris Corpus. French Language Studies 22(1): 7–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgenstern, A., Leroy, M. & Caët S. 2013. Self- and other-repairs in child-adult interaction at the intersection of pragmatic abilities and language acquisition. Journal of Pragmatics 56: 151–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E. 2007. The effect of bilingualism on the use of manual gestures. Applied Psycholinguistics 28: 441–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Mayberry R. J. & Genesee, F. 1999. Gesture and early bilingual development. Developmental Psychology 35(2): 514–526. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kimbrough Oller, D. & Jarmulowicz, L. 2007. Language and literacy in bilingual children in the early school years. In Handbook of Language Development, E. Hoff & M. Shatz (eds), 368–386. Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Parisse, C. & Morgenstern, A. 2012. The unfolding of the verbal temporal system in 2 French children’s speech between 18 and 36 months. French Language Studies 22(1): 95–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pea, R. D. 1980. The development of negation in early child language. In The Social Foundations of Language and Thought: Essays in Honor of Jerome Seymour Bruner, D. R. Olson (ed.), 156–186. New York NY: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Sekali, M. 2012. The emergence of complex sentences in a French child’s language from 0;10 to 4;01: Causal adverbial clauses and the concertina effect. Journal of French Language Studies 22: 115–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shaw, E. 2013. Gesture in Multiparty Interaction: A Study of Embodied Discourse in Spoken English and American Sign Language. PhD dissertation, Georgetown University.Google Scholar
Spitz, R. A. 1957. No and Yes: On the Genesis of Human Communication. New York NY: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Stern, C. & Stern, W. 1907[1928]. Die Kindersprache – Eine psychologische Untersuchung. Leipzig: Barth.Google Scholar
Streeck, J. 2009. Gesturecraft: The Manu-facture of Meaning [Gesture Studies 2]. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sutton-Spence, R. & Woll, B. 1999. The Linguistics of British Sign Language. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 2003. Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tuller L., Blondel, M. & Niederberger N. 2007. Growing up in French and French Sign Language. In French Applied Linguistics [Language Learnng and Language Teaching 16], D. Ayoun (ed.), 334–376. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vaidyanathan, R. 1991. Development of forms and functions of negation in the early stages of language acquisition: A study in Tamil. Journal of Child Language 18: 51–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V. & Antinucci, F. 1979. Negation in child language: A pragmatic study. In Developmental pragmatics, E. Ochs & B. B. Schieffelin (eds), 281–303. New York NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Volterra, V., Caselli, M. C., Capirci, O., Pizzuto, E. 2004. Gesture and the emergence and development of language. In Beyond Nature-Nurture: Essays in Honor of Elisabeth Bates, M. Tomasello & D. I. Slobin (eds), 22–44. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Wode, H. 1977. Four early stages in the development of L1 negation. Journal of Child Language 4: 87–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zeshan, U. 2006. Negative and interrogative constructions in sign languages: A case study in sign language typology. In Interrogative and Negative Constructions in Sign Languages [Sign Language Typology 1], U. Zeshan (ed.), 28–68. Nijmegen: Ishara Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zlatev, J., Persson T. & Gärdenfors, P. 2005. Bodily mimesis as “the missing link”. In Human Cognitive Evolution [LUCS 121]. Lund: Lund University Cognitive Studies.Google Scholar
Cited by (6)

Cited by six other publications

Harrison, Simon
2024. On Grammar–Gesture Relations: Gestures Associated with Negation. In The Cambridge Handbook of Gesture Studies,  pp. 446 ff. DOI logo
Morgenstern, Aliyah
2024. Gesture and First Language Development: The Multimodal Child. In The Cambridge Handbook of Gesture Studies,  pp. 368 ff. DOI logo
Boutet, Dominique, Marion Blondel, Pauline Beaupoil-Hourdel & Aliyah Morgenstern
2021. A multimodal and kinesiological approach to the development of negation in signing and non-signing children. Languages and Modalities 1  pp. 31 ff. DOI logo
Harrison, Simon & Silva H. Ladewig
Morgenstern, Aliyah, Lea Chevrefils, Marion Blondel, Coralie Vincent, Chloé Thomas, Jean-François Jego & Dominique Boutet
2021. “Of thee I sing”: An opening to Dominique Boutet’s kinesiological approach to gesture. Languages and Modalities 1  pp. 3 ff. DOI logo
Benazzo, Sandra & Aliyah Morgenstern
2014. A bilingual child’s multimodal path into negation. Gesture 14:2  pp. 171 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 27 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.