Part of
The Acquisition of Referring Expressions: A dialogical approach
Edited by Anne Salazar-Orvig, Geneviève de Weck, Rouba Hassan and Annie Rialland
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 28] 2021
► pp. 262286
References
Allen, S. E. M.
(2000) A discourse-pragmatic explanation for argument representation in child Inuktitut. Linguistics, 38, 483–521. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Altinkamis, N. F., Kern, S., & Sofu, H.
(2014) When context matters more than language: Verbs or nouns in French and Turkish caregiver speech. First language, 34(6), 537–550. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barton, M., & Tomasello, M.
(1994) The rest of the family: The role of fathers and siblings in early language development. In C. Gallaway & B. Richard (Eds.) Input and interaction in language acquisition (pp. 109–134). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A., & Nir-Sagiv, B.
(2004) Linguistic indicators of inter-genre differentiation in later language development. Journal of Child Language, 31(2), 339–380. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blewitt, P., Rump, K. M., Shealy, S. E., & Cook, S. A.
(2009) Shared book reading: When and how questions affect young children’s word learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 294–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boiron, V.
(2010) La compréhension du récit de fiction en petite section: Développement, apprentissage et perspectives didactiques. Le Français Aujourd’hui, 179, 67–84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bronckart, J.-P.
(1996) Activité langagière, textes et discours. Pour un interactionisme socio-discursif. Lausanne: Delachaux et Niestlé.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S.
(1983) Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New York, NY: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
da Silva-Genest, C., Marcos, H., Salazar Orvig, A., Caët, S., & Heurdier, J.
(2021) Young children’s uses of referring expressions. In A. Salazar Orvig, G. de Weck, R. Hassan, & A. Rialland (Eds.), The acquisition of referring expressions: A dialogical approach (pp. XXX–YYY). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. (This volume). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Weck, G., & Jullien, S.
(2013) How do French-speaking children with specific language impairment first mention referent in storytelling? Between reference and grammar. Journal of Pragmatics, 56, 70–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Weck, G. & Rosat, M.-C.
(2003) Troubles dysphasiques. Comment raconter, relater, faire agir à l’âge pré-scolaire. Paris: Masson.Google Scholar
Dolz, J., Pasquier, A., & Bronckart, J.-P.
(1993) L’acquisition des discours: Émergence d’une compétence ou apprentissage de capacités langagières diverses? Etudes de Linguistique Appliquée, 92, 23–37.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S.
(2005) Impact du cadre interactionnel sur les acquisitions en syntaxe. Acquisition et Interaction en Langue Étrangère, 4, 53–80.Google Scholar
Gallaway, C., & Richards, B. J.
(1994) Input and interaction in language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gee, J., & Savasir, I.
(1985) On the use of will and gonna: Toward a description of activity-types for child language. Discourse Processes, 8(2), 143–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gerde, H. K., & Powell, D. R.
(2009) Teacher education, book-reading practices, and children’s language growth across one year of head start. Early Education and Development, 20(2), 211–237. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grossman, F.
(1996) Enfances de la lecture: Manières de faire, manières de lire à l’école maternelle. Berne: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Hassan, R., de Weck, G., Rezzonico, S., Salazar Orvig, A., & Vinel, E.
(2021) Variations in adult use of referring expressions during storytelling in different interactional settings. In A. Salazar Orvig, G. de Weck, R. Hassan, & A. Rialland (Eds.), The acquisition of referring expressions: A dialogical approach (pp. XXX–YYY). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. (This volume). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hassan, R., Vinel, E., & Salagnac, N.
(2013) Étude comparative des interactions verbales dans une activité de récit dialogué en milieux familial et scolaire, 12e colloque international de l’AIRDF, L’enseignement du français à l’ère numérique, Haute École Pédagogique de Lausanne 29–31 août.Google Scholar
Heurdier, J.
(2015) Usages syntaxiques et dialogue parent-enfant. Etude de dyades mère-enfant et père-enfant dans deux activités ludiques (PhD dissertation). Université Sorbonne Nouvelle – Paris 3, Paris, France.Google Scholar
Hickmann, M.
(2003) Children’s discourse. Person, space and time across languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hoff, E.
(2010) Context effects on young children’s language use: The influence of conversational setting and partner. First language, 30(3–4), 461–472. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hosmer, D. W., Lemeshow, S., & Sturdivant, R. X.
(2013) Assessing the Fit of the Model. In D. W. Hosmer, S. Lemeshow & R. X. Sturdivant (Eds.), Applied logistic regression (pp. 153–226). New York, NY: John Wiley & Son.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hudelot, C.
(2007) The use of a functional dialogic model of verbal interaction to compare how daycare workers and teachers scaffold 3-year-old children. In L. Berlin (Ed.), Theoretical approaches to dialogue analysis (pp. 215–227). Tübingen: Max Niemeyer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hughes, M., & Allen, S. E. M.
(2013) The effect of individual discourse-pragmatic features on referential choice in child English. Journal of Pragmatics, 56, 15–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kail, M., & Hickmann, M.
(1992) On French children’s ability to introduce referents in discourse as a function of mutual knowledge. First Language, 12, 73–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kern, S., & Chenu, F.
(2010) Contextes de production et LAE en français: caractéristiques pragmatiques et syntaxiques. In F. Neveu, V. Muni Toke, T. Klingler, J. Durand, L. Mondada, & S. Prévost (Eds.), Actes du congrès mondial de linguistique française (pp. 1527–1537). Paris: Institut de Linguistique Française. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kern, S., & Raffara, A.
(2012) Effet du type de support imagé sur la production du récit chez l’enfant. In R. Delamotte-Legrand & M.-A. Akinci (Eds.), Récits d’enfants: Développement, genre, contexte (pp. 97–115). Rouen: Publications des Universités de Rouen et du Havre.Google Scholar
Leaper, C., & Gleason, J. B.
(1996) The relationship of play activity and gender to parent and child sex-typed communication. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 19(4), 689–703. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C.
(1979) Activity types and language. Linguistics, 17, 365–399. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nashawati, S.
(2010) Le développement des expressions référentielles chez le jeune enfant: Noms et pronoms dans des dialogues mère-enfant (PhD dissertation). Université Sorbonne Nouvelle – Paris 3, Paris, France. [URL]
Rezzonico, S., Vinel, E., de Weck, G., Hassan, R., & Salagnac, N.
(2021) Referring in dialogical narratives: A study on children’s use of nouns and pronouns. In A. Salazar Orvig, G. de Weck, R. Hassan, & A. Rialland (Eds.), The acquisition of referring expressions: A dialogical approach (pp. 115–139). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. (This volume). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salazar Orvig, A.
(2002) Remarques sur la continuité et l’engagement dialogique chez le jeune enfant. In L. Danon-Boileau, C. Hudelot, & A. Salazar Orvig (Eds.), Usages du langage chez l’enfant (pp. 61–81). Paris: Ophrys.Google Scholar
Salazar Orvig, A., Marcos, H., Heurdier, J., & da Silva-Genest, C.
(2018) Referential features, speech genres and activity types. In M. Hickmann, E. Veneziano, & H. Jisa (Eds.), Sources of variation in first language acquisition: Languages, contexts, and learners (pp. 219–242). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salazar Orvig, A., Marcos, H., Morgenstern, A., Hassan, R., Leber-Marin, J., & Parès, J.
(2010) Dialogical beginnings of anaphora: The use of third person pronouns before the age of 3. Journal of Pragmatics, 42(7), 1842–1865. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L.
(2008) The role of discourse and perceptual cues in the choice of referential expressions in english preschoolers, school-age children, and adults. Language Learning and Development, 4(4), 309–332. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siegel, S. S., & Castellan Jr., N. J.
(1988) Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Spurrier, J. D.
(2003) On the null distribution of the Kruskal-Wallis statistic. Journal of Nonparametric Statistics, 15(6), 685–691. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E.
(2000) Interaction, conversation et acquisition du langage dans les trois premières années. In M. Kail & M. Fayol (Eds.), L’acquisition du langage. Le langage en émergence. De la naissance à trois ans (Vol. I., pp. 231–265). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vinel, E.
(2014) Comment des adultes et des enfants, âgés de 3 à 6 ans, racontent ensemble des histoires en situations familiales et scolaires (PhD dissertation). Université Sorbonne Nouvelle – Paris 3, Paris, France. [URL]
Vinel, E., Salagnac, N., & Hassan, R.
(2014) Expressions référentielles et reprises d’énoncés dans des dialogues adulte-enfant à l’école et en famille: une étude exploratoire. TRANEL, 60, 33–45.Google Scholar
Vinel, E., Salazar Orvig, A., de Weck, G., Nashawati, S., & Rahmati, S.
(2021) The impact of speech genres on the use of referring expressions. In A. Salazar Orvig, G. de Weck, R. Hassan, & A. Rialland (Eds.), The acquisition of referring expressions: A dialogical approach (pp. XXX–YYY). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. (This volume). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S.
(1933/1962) Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wittek, A., & Tomasello, M.
(2005) Young children’s sensitivity to listener knowledge and perceptual context in choosing referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 541–558. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zucker, T. A., Justice, L. M., Piasta, S. B., & Kaderavek, J. N.
(2010) Preschool teachers’ literal and inferential questions and children’s responses during whole-class shared reading. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 65–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar