Part of
Colour Studies: A broad spectrum
Edited by Wendy Anderson, Carole P. Biggam, Carole Hough and Christian Kay
[Not in series 191] 2014
► pp. 200211
References (31)
Alvarez, James, Alexandra Clifford, Amanda Holmes, and Anna Franklin. Under review. “The ‘Lateralized Whorf’ effect is not necessarily linguistic or categorical.”
Bornstein, Marc H., and Nancy O. Korda. 1984. “Discrimination and Matching within and between Hues Measured by Reaction Times: Some Implications for Categorical Perception and Levels of Information Processing.” Psychological Research 46 (3): 207–222. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, Angela M., Lindsey Delwin, and Kevin M. Guckes. 2011. “Color Names, Color Categories and Color–cued Visual Search: Sometimes, Color Perception is not Categorical.” Journal of Vision 11: 1–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, Roger W., and Eric H. Lenneberg. 1954. “A study in language and cognition.” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 49 (3): 454–462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Daoutis, Christine, Michael Pilling, and Ian Davies. 2006. “Categorical Effects in Visual Search for Colour.” Visual Cognition 14: 217–240. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Derrington, Andrew M., John Krauskopf, and Peter Lennie. 1984. “Chromatic Mechanisms in the Lateral Geniculate Nucleus of Macaque.” Journal of Physiology 357: 241–265.Google Scholar
Deutscher, Guy. 2011. Through the Language Glass: Why the World Looks Different in Other Languages. London: Random House.Google Scholar
Drivonikou, Gilda V., Paul Kay, Terry Regier, Richard B. Ivry, Aubrey L. Gilbert, Anna Franklin, and Ian R.L. Davies. 2007. “Further Evidence that Whorfian Effects are Stronger in the Right Visual Field than the Left.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104 (3): 1097–1102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fairchild, Mark D. 1998. Color Appearance Models. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Gegenfurtner, Karl Reiner, and Daniel C. Kiper. 2003. “Color Vision.” Annual Review of Neuroscience 26 (1): 181–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, Aubrey L., Terry Regier, Paul Kay, and Richard B. Ivry. 2006. “Whorf Hypothesis is Supported in the Right Visual Field but not in the Left.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 103 (2): 489–494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heinrich, S.P., K. Kruger, and M. Bach. 2011. “The Dynamics of Practice Effects in an Optotype Acuity Task.” Graefes Archives for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology 249 (9): 1319–1326. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huette, S., and B. McMurray. 2010. “Continuous Dynamics of Color Categorization.” Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 17 (3): 348–354. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kay, Paul, and Willett Kempton. 1984. “What is the Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis?American Anthropologist 86: 65–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krauskopf, John, and Karl Reiner Gegenfurtner. 1992. “Color Discrimination and Adaptation.” Vision Research 32 (11): 2165–2175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krauskopf, John, David R. Williams, and David W. Heeley. 1982. “Cardinal Directions of Color Space.” Vision Research 22 (9): 1123–1131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P.K. 1991. “Human Adults and Human Infants Show a ‘perceptual magnet effect’ for the Prototypes of Speech Categories, Monkeys do not.” Perception and psychophysics 50 (2): 93–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lindsey, Delwin T., Angela M. Brown, Ester Reijnen, Anina N. Rich, Yoana I. Kuzmova, and Jeremy M. Wolfe. 2010. “Color Channels, not Color Appearance or Color Categories, Guide Visual Search for Desaturated Color Targets.” Psychological Science 21 (9): 1208–1214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lu, Z.L., T. Hua, C.B. Huang, Y. Zhou, and B.A. Dosher. 2011. “Visual perceptual learning.” Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 95 (2): 145–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Olkkonen, Maria, Christoph Witzel, Thorsten Hansen, and Karl Reiner Gegenfurtner. 2010. “Categorical Color Constancy for Real Surfaces.” Journal of Vision 10 (9) 16: 1–22. [URL] DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raskin, L.A., S. Maital, and Marc H. Bornstein. 1983. “Perceptual Categorization of Color: a Life-span Study.” Psychological Research 45 (2): 135–145. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Regier, Terry, Paul Kay, and Naveen Khetarpal. 2007. “Color Naming Reflects Optimal Partitions of Color Space.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104 (4): 1436–1441. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberson, Debi, J. Richard Hanley, and Hyensou Pak. 2009. “Thresholds for Color Discrimination in English and Korean Speakers.” Cognition 112 (3): 482–487. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberson, Debi, Ian R.L. Davies, and Jules Davidoff. 2000. “Color Categories are not Universal: Replications and New Evidence from a Stone-age Culture.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 129 (3): 369–398. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Winawer, J., N. Witthoft, M.C. Frank, L. Wu, A.R. Wade, and L. Boroditsky. 2007. “Russian Blues Reveal Effects of Language on Color Discrimination.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104 (19): 7780-7785. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Witzel, Christoph, and Karl Reiner Gegenfurtner. 2011. “Is there a Lateralized Category Effect for Color?Journal of Vision 11 (12) 16: 1–25. [URL] DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2012. “Category Effects for Red and Brown.” Perception 41 ECVP Abstract Supplement: 11.Google Scholar
. 2013. “Categorical Sensitivity to Colour Differences.” Journal of Vision 13 (7) 1: 1–33. [URL] DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. Under review. “Categorical Facilitation Effects for Equally Discriminable Colours.”
Witzel, Christoph, Thorsten Hansen, and Karl Reiner Gegenfurtner. 2008. “Categorical Discrimination of Colour.” Journal of Vision 8 (6): 577. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2009. “Categorical Reaction Times for Equally Discriminable Colours.” Perception 38 ECVP Abstract Supplement: 14.Google Scholar